24
Views
31
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

The Acquisition of Time Properties Associated with a Sequential Motor Skill

&
Pages 275-301 | Received 15 Dec 1982, Published online: 13 Aug 2013

References

  • Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 3, 111–150.
  • Armstrong, T. R. (1970). Training for the production of memorized movement patterns.(Technical Report No. 26). Human Performance Center, University of Michigan.
  • Battig, W. (1966). Facilitation and interference. In E. Bilodeau (Ed.), Acquisition of skill(pp. 215–244). New York: Academic Press.
  • Battig, W. (1972). Intratask interference as a source of facilitation in transfer and retention. In R. Thompson & J. Voss (Eds.), Topics in learning and performance (pp. 131–159). New York: Academic.
  • Battig, W. (1979). The flexibility of human memory. In L. Cermak & F. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing in human memory (pp. 23–44). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  • Denier van der Gon, J. J., & Thuring, J. Ph. (1965). The guiding of human writing movements. Kybernetik, 2, 145–148.
  • Fowler, C. A., & Turvey, M. T. (1978). Skill acquisition: An event approach with special reference to searching for the optimum of a function of several variables. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 1–40). New York: Academic Press.
  • Fraisse, P. (1981). Multisensory aspects of rhythm. In R. Walk & H. L. Pick (Eds.), Intersensory perception and sensory integration (pp. 217–247). New York: Plenum.
  • Fuchs, A. (1962). The progression hypothesis in perceptual-motor skill learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 177–182.
  • Gelfand, I. M., & Tsetlin, M. (1971). Mathematical modeling of mechanisms of the central nervous system. In I. M. Gelfand, V. Gurfinkel, S. Fomin, & M. Tsetlin (Eds.), Models of the structural-functional organization of certain biological systems (pp. 1–22). Cambridge: M. I. T. Press.
  • Getner, D. (1982). Evidence against a central control model of timing in typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 793–810.
  • Holding, D. (1965). Principles of training. New York: Pergamon.
  • Hollerbach, J. M. (1981). An oscillation theory of handwriting. Biological Cybernetics, 39, 139–156.
  • Kelso, J. A. S. (1982). Human motor behavior: An introduction. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  • Kelso, J. A. S., Southard, D. L. & Goodman, D. (1979). On the coordination of two-handed aiming movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 229–238.
  • Kelso, J. A. S., Putnam, C., & Goodman, D. (1983). On the space-time structure of human interlimb coordination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 347–375.
  • Lee, T. D. & Magill, R. A. (1983). The locus of contextual interference in motor-skill acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 9, 730–746.
  • Marteniuk, R., & Romanow, S. (1983). Human movement organization as revealed by velocity of movement, use of kinematic information, and Fourier analysis. In R. Magill (Ed.), Memory and control on action (pp. 167–197). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
  • Pew, R. (1974). Human perceptual-motor performance. In B. H. Kantowitz (Ed.), Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition (pp. 1–39). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  • Povel, D. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3–18.
  • Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.
  • Schmidt, R. A. (1982). Motor control and learning. Champaign: Human Kinetics.
  • Shapiro, D. (1977). A preliminary attempt to determine the duration of a motor program. In D. M. Landers and R. W. Christina (Eds.), Psychology of motor behavior and sport (pp. 17–24). Urbana, IL: Human Kinetics.
  • Shapiro, D. (1978). The learning of generalized motor programs. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Angeles.
  • Shapiro, D. (1983, June). Motor learning and generalized motor programs. In J. A. S. Kelso (chair), The learning of skilled movements. Symposium conducted at the annual meeting, North American Society for the Psychology of Sport and Physical Activity, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan.
  • Shapiro, D., & Schmidt, R. A. (1982). The schema theory: Recent evidence and developmental implications. In J. A. Kelso & J. E. Clark (Eds.), The development of movement control and coordination (pp. 113–149). New York: Wiley.
  • Shea, J., & Morgan, R. (1979). Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 179–187.
  • Soechting, J., & Lacquaniti, F. (1981). Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man. Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 710–720.
  • Summers, J. J. (1975). The role of timing in motor program representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 229–241.
  • Terzuolo, C, & Viviani, P. (1979). The central representation of learned motor patterns. In R. Talbott and D. Humphrey (Eds.), Posture and movement (pp. 113–121). New York: Raven Press.
  • Thorndike, E. L. (1903). Educational psychology. New York: Lemcke & Buechner.
  • Turvey, M. T., Shaw, R., & Mace, W. (1978). Issues in the theory of action: Degrees of freedom, coordinative structures, and coalitions. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance VII (pp. 557–595). Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.
  • Viviani, P., & Terzuolo, C. (1980). Space-time invariance in learned motor skills. In G. Stelmach and J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 525–533). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
  • Viviani, P., & Terzuolo, C. (1982). Trajectory determines movement dynamics. Neuroscience, 7, 431–437.
  • Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1978). On the temporal control of rhythmic performance. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance VII (pp. 535–555). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  • Wing, A. (1977). Effects of type of movement on the temporal precision of response sequences. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 30, 60–72.
  • Wing, A. (1980a). The long and short of timing in response sequences. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 469–486). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
  • Wing, A. M. (1980b). The height of handwriting. Acta Psychologica, 46, 141–151.
  • Wing, A., & Kristofferson, A. (1973). Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 5–12.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.