35
Views
53
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Effects on Prism Adaptation of Duration and Timing of Visual Feedback During Pointing

&
Pages 209-224 | Received 18 Jan 1989, Published online: 13 Aug 2013

References

  • Annett, J., Golby, C. W., & Kay, H. (1958). The measurements of elements in an assembly task: The information output of the human motor system. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 1–11.
  • Auerbach, C., & Sperling, P. (1974). A common auditory-visual space: Evidence for its reality. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 129–135.
  • Bedford, F. L. (1989). Constraints on learning new mapping between perceptual dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 232–248.
  • Beggs, W. D. A., & Howarth, C. I. (1972). The movement of the hand toward a target. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 448–453.
  • Craske, B. (1975). A current view of the processes and mechanisms of prism adaptation. Les Colloques de I'Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, 43, 125–138.
  • Crawshaw, M., & Craske, B. (1974). No retinal component in prism adaptation. Acta Psychologica, 38, 421–423.
  • Fisher, G. H. (1962). Intersensory localization. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Hull, England.
  • Epstein, W. (1975). Recalibration by pairing: A process of perceptual learning. Perception, 4, 59–72.
  • Goodale, M. A., Pelisson, D., & Prablanc, C. (1986). Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature, 320, 748–750.
  • Harris, C. S. (1974). Beware of the straight-ahead shift: A nonperceptual change in experiments on adaptation to displaced vision. Perception, 3, 461–476.
  • Harris, C. S. (1980). Insight or out of sight? Two examples of perceptual plasticity in the human adult. In C. S. Harris (Ed.), Visual coding and adaptability. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Hay, J. C., & Pick, H. L., Jr. (1966). Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 150–158.
  • Hay, J. C., Pick, H. L., Jr., & Ikeda, K. (1965). Visual capture produced by prism spectacles. Psychonomic Science, 2, 215–216.
  • Held, R., & Bossom, J. (1961). Neonatal deprivation and adult rearrangement: Complementary techniques for analyzing plastic sensory-motor coordinations. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 54, 33–37.
  • Howard, I. P. (1971). Perceptual learning and adaptation. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 248–252.
  • Howard, I. P., (1982). Human visual orientation. New York: Wiley.
  • Jeannerod, M. (1984). The timing of natural prehension movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 235–254.
  • Keele, S. W. (1981). Behavioral analysis of movement. In V. B. Brooks (Ed.), Handbook of Physiology, Sec. 1. The nervous system, Vol.2. Motor control, Pt. 2 (pp. 1391–1414). Baltimore, MD: American Physiological Society.
  • McCloskey, D. I. (1981). Corollary discharges: Motor commands and perception. In V. B. Brooks (Ed.), Handbook of physiology: Sec. 1. The nervous system; Vol. 2. Motor control, Pt. 2 (pp. 1415–1447). Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society.
  • Paillard, J. (1982). The contribution of peripheral and central vision to visually guided reaching. In D. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & M. A. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 367–385). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Redding, G. M. (1978). Additivity in adaptation to optical tilt. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 178–190.
  • Redding, G. M. (1981). Effects of homogenous and variable exposure on magnitude of adaptation to optical tilt. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 130–140.
  • Redding, G. M., Clark, S. E., & Wallace, B. (1985). Attention and prism adaptation. Cognitive Psyctofogy, 17, 1–25.
  • Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1976). Components of displacement adaptation in acquisition and decay as a function of hand and hall exposure. Perception and Psychophysics, 20, 453–459.
  • Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1978). Sources of “overadditivity” in prism adaptation. Perception and Psychophysics, 24, 58–62.
  • Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1985a). Cognitive interference in prism adaptation. Perception and Psychophysics, 37, 225–230.
  • Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1985b). Perceptual-motor coordination and adaptation during locomotion: Determinants of prism adaptation in hall exposure. Perception and Psychophysics, 38, 320–330.
  • Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1987). Perceptual-motor coordination and adaptation during locomotion: A control for head posture effects. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 269–274.
  • Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1988a). Components of prism adaptation in terminal and concurrent exposure: Organization of the eye-hand coordination loop. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 59–68.
  • Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1988b). Adaptive mechanisms in perceptual-motor coordination: Components of prism adaptation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 20, 242–254.
  • Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1988c). Head posture effects in prism adaptation during hallway exposure. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 69–75.
  • Robinson, D. A. (1976). Adaptive gain control of vestibuloocular reflex by the cerebellum. Journal of Neuropsychology, 39, 954–969.
  • Templeton, W. B., Howard, I. P., & Wilkinson, D. A. (1974). Additivity of components of prismatic adaptation. Perception and Psychophysics, 15, 249–257.
  • Uhlarik, J. J., & Canon, L. K. (1971). Influence of concurrent and terminal exposure conditions on the nature of perceptual adaptation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9, 233–239.
  • Wallace, B. (1977). Stability of Wilkinson's linear model of prism adaptation over time for various targets. Perception, 6, 145–151.
  • Wallace, B. (1978). Visuomotor coordination and intermanual transfer for a proprioceptive reaching task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 10, 139–147.
  • Wallace, B., & Redding, G. M. (1979). Additivity in prism adaptation as manifested in intermanual and interocular transfer. Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 133–136.
  • Welch, R. B. (1974). Research on adaptation to rearranged vision: 1966-1974. Perception, 3, 367–392.
  • Welch, R. B., Choe, C. S., & Heinrich, D. R. (1974). Evidence for a three-component model of prism adaptation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 700–705.
  • Welch, R. B., & Warren, D. H. (1980). Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 638–667.
  • Wilkinson, D. A. (1971). Visual-motor control loop: A linear system? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 250–257.
  • Woodworth, R. S. (1898). The accuracy of voluntary movements. Psychological Review, Monograph Supp. 3, Whole No. 114.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.