92
Views
16
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
RESEARCH ARTICLES

Effects of Mental Footnotes on the Trajectory Movement in a Driving Simulation Task

Pages 211-225 | Received 02 Jan 2014, Accepted 29 Sep 2014, Published online: 25 Nov 2014

REFERENCES

  • Braine, M. D. S., & O’Brien, D. (1998). Mental logic. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Buckingham, G., Cant, J. S., & Goodale, M. A. (2009). Living in a material world: How visual cues to material properties affect the way that we lift objects and perceive their weight. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 3111–3118.
  • Buckingham, G., & Goodale, M. A. (2010). The influence of competing perceptual and motor priors in the context of the size–weight illusion. Experimental Brain Research, 205, 283–288.
  • Carpenter, W. B. (1852). On the influence of suggestion in modifying and directing muscular movement, independently of volition. Proceedings of the Royal Institution of Great Britain, 1, 147–153.
  • Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology. London, England: Henry S. King.
  • Castro, C., Moreno-Ríos, S., Tornay, F., & Vargas, C. (2008). Mental representations of obligatory and prohibitory traffic signs. Acta Psychologica, 129, 8–17.
  • Clark, H. H., & Chase, W. G. (1972). On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 472–517.
  • De Vega, M., Moreno, V., & Castillo, D. (2013). The comprehension of action-related sentences may cause interference rather than facilitation on matching actions. Psychological Research, 77, 20–30.
  • Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance performance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 481–492.
  • Frank, C., Land, W. M., & Schack, T. (2013). Mental representation and learning: the influence of practice on the development of mental representation structure in complex action. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14, 353–361.
  • Ganel, T., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape. Nature, 426, 664–667.
  • Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.
  • Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1999). Attention and the control of action: An investigation of the effects of selection on population coding of hand and eye movement. In D. Heinke, G. W. Humphreys, & A. Olsen (Eds.), Connectionist models in cognitive neuroscience: Proceedings of the 5th neural computational and psychological workshop (pp. 283–298). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Toward a cognitive science of language, inference and consciousness. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2006). How we reason. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Byrne, R. M. (2002). Conditionals: A theory of meaning, pragmatics and inferences. Psychological Review, 109, 646–678.
  • Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Variable action effects: Response control by context-specific effect anticipations. Psychological Research, 68, 155–162.
  • Lee, D. (1999). Effects of exogenous and endogenous attention on visually guided hand Movements. Cognitive Brain Research, 8, 143–156.
  • Macdonald, W. A., & Hoffmann, E. R. (1978). Information coding on turn restriction signs. Proceedings of the 9th ARRB Conference, 9, 361–382.
  • Magill, R. A. (2011). Motor learning and control: Concepts and applications. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • Motowidlo, S. J., & Van Scotter, J. R. (1994). Evidence that task performance should be distinguished from contextual performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 475–480.
  • Pfister, R., Melcher, T., Kiesel, A., Dechent, P., & Gruber, O. (2014). Neural correlates of ideomotor effect anticipations. Neuroscience, 259, 164–171.
  • Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.
  • Stock, A., & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research, 68, 176–188.
  • Tipper, S. P., Howard, L. A., & Jackson, S. R. (1997). Selective reaching to grasp: Evidence for distractor interference effects. Visual Cognition, 4, 1–38.
  • Vilchez, J. L. (2013). The locus of stimuli meaning in the influence of attention on movement: Meaning-dependent activation model. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 34, 165–176.
  • Vilchez, J. L., & Tornay, F. J. (2012). Irrelevant stimuli produce a path deviation in a driving-simulation task. Cognitive Systems Research, 17–18, 81–89.
  • Welsh, T. N. (2011). The relationship between attentional capture and deviations in movement trajectories in selective reaching tasks. Acta Psychologica, 137, 300–308.
  • Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2004). Movement trajectories in the presence of a distracting stimulus: Evidence for a response activation model of selective reaching. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1031–1057.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.