251
Views
9
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Fitts’ Theorem in Oculomotor Control: Dissociable Movement Times for Amplitude and Width Manipulations

, , &
Pages 489-499 | Received 17 Aug 2015, Accepted 29 Nov 2015, Published online: 24 Jun 2016

REFERENCES

  • Abrams, R. A., Meyer, D. E., & Kornblum, S. (1989). Speed and accuracy of saccadic eye movements: Characteristics of impulse variability in the oculomotor system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 529.
  • Bahill, A. T., Clark, M. R., & Stark, L. (1975). Dynamic overshoot in saccadic eye movements is caused by neurological control signed reversals. Experimental Neurology, 48, 107–122.
  • Becker, W., & Fuchs, A. F. (1969). Further properties of the human saccadic system: Eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Vision Research, 9, 1247–1258.
  • Beggs, W. D. A., Graham, J. C., Monk, T. H., Shaw, M. R. W., & Howarth, C. I. (1972). Can Hick's law and Fitts' law be combined? Acta Psychologica, 36, 348–357.
  • Bell, A. H., Everling, S., & Munoz, D. P. (2000). Influence of stimulus eccentricity and direction on characteristics of pro- and antisaccades in non-human primates. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, 2595–2604.
  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.
  • Bridgeman, B., & Fisher, B. (1990). Saccadic suppression of displacement is strongest in central vision. Perception, 19, 103–111.
  • Carmer, S. G., & Seif, R. D. (1963). Calculation of orthogonal coefficients when treatments are unequally replicated and/or unequally spaced. Agronomy Journal, 55, 387–389.
  • Chi, C. F., & Lin, C. L. (1997). Speed and accuracy of eye-gaze pointing. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 705–718.
  • Chua, R., & Elliott, D. (1993). Visual regulation of manual aiming. Human Movement Science, 12, 365–401.
  • Collewijn, H., Erkelens, C. J., & Steinman, R. M. (1988). Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. Journal of Physiology, 404, 157–182.
  • Cumming, G. (2013). Understanding the new statistics: Effect sizes, confidence intervals, and meta-analysis. New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Dafoe, J. M., Armstrong, I. T., & Munoz, D. P. (2007). The influence of stimulus direction and eccentricity on pro- and anti-saccades in humans. Experimental Brain Research, 179, 563–570.
  • de Grosbois, J., Heath, M., & Tremblay, L. (2015). Augmented feedback influences upper limb reaching movement times but does not explain violations of Fitts' law. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 800.
  • DeSimone, J. C., Everling, S., & Heath, M. (2015). The antisaccade task: Visual distractors elicit a location-independent planning 'cost'. PloS One, 10, e0122345.
  • DeSimone, J. C., Weiler, J., Aber, G. S., & Heath, M. (2014). The unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost: Correct and error antisaccades differentially influence the planning times for subsequent prosaccades. Vision Research, 96C, 17–24.
  • Ditterich, J., Eggert, T., & Straube, A. (1998). Fixation errors and timing in sequences of memory-guided saccades. Behavioural Brain Research, 95, 205–217.
  • Elliott, D., Carson, R. G., Goodman, D., & Chua, R. (1991). Discrete vs. Continuous visual control of manual aiming. Human Movement Science, 10, 393–418.
  • Elliott, D., Hansen, S., Grierson, L. E., Lyons, J., Bennett, S. J., & Hayes, S. J. (2010). Goal-directed aiming: Two components but multiple processes. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1023–1044.
  • Elliott, D., Helsen, W. F., & Chua, R. (2001). A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 342–357.
  • Evdokimidis, I., Tsekou, H., & Smyrnis, N. (2006). The mirror antisaccade task: Direction-amplitude interaction and spatial accuracy characteristics. Experimental Brain Research, 174, 304–311.
  • Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 47, 381–391.
  • Fitts, P. M., & Peterson, J. R. (1964). Information capacity of discrete motor responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 103–112.
  • Flanders, M., Helms Tillery, S. I., & Soechting, J. F. (1992). Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 309–362.
  • Gillen, C., & Heath, M. (2014). Perceptual averaging governs antisaccade endpoint bias. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 3201–3210.
  • Gillen, C., Weiler, J., & Heath, M. (2013). Stimulus-driven saccades are characterized by an invariant undershooting bias: No evidence for a range effect. Experimental Brain Research, 230, 165–167.
  • Harris, C. M., & Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature, 394, 780–784.
  • Harris, C. M., & Wolpert, D. M. (2006). The main sequence of saccades optimizes speed-accuracy trade-off. Biological Cybernetics, 95, 21–29.
  • Heath, M., Gillen, C., & Weiler, J. (2015). The antisaccade task: Vector inversion contributes to a statistical summary representation of target eccentricities. Journal of Vision, 15, 1–13.
  • Heath, M., Hassall, C. D., MacLean, S., & Krigolson, O. E. (2015). Event-related brain potentials during the visuomotor mental rotation task: The contingent negative variation scales to angle of rotation. Neuroscience, 311, 153–165.
  • Heath, M., Weiler, J., Marriott, K., & Welsh, T. N. (2011). Vector inversion diminishes the online control of antisaccades. Experimental Brain Research, 209, 117–127.
  • Heath, M., Weiler, J., Marriott, K. A., Elliott, D., & Binsted, G. (2011). Revisiting Fitts and Peterson (1964): Width and amplitude manipulations to the reaching environment elicit dissociable movement times. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 259–268.
  • Jagacinski, R. J., Repperger, D. W., Ward, S. L., & Moran, M. S. (1980). A test of Fitts' law with moving targets. Human Factors, 22, 225–233.
  • Kalesnykas, R. P., & Hallett, P. E. (1994). Retinal eccentricity and the latency of eye saccades. Vision Research, 34, 517–531.
  • Klapp, S. T. (1975). Feedback versus motor programming in the control of aimed movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 104, 161–169.
  • Klapp, S. T., & Erwin, C. I. (1976). Relation between programming time and duration of the response being programmed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 591–598.
  • Krappmann, P., Everling, S., & Flohr, H. (1998). Accuracy of visually and memory-guided antisaccades in man. Vision Research, 38, 2979–2985.
  • Kveraga, K., Boucher, L., & Hughes, H. C. (2002). Saccades operate in violation of Hick's law. Experimental Brain Research, 146, 307–314.
  • Langolf, G. D., Chaffin, D. B., & Foulke, J. A. (1976). An investigation of Fitts’ law using a wide range of movement amplitudes. Journal of Motor Behavior, 8, 113–128.
  • Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490.
  • MacKenzie, C. L., & Graham, E. D. (1997). Separate A and W effects: Pointing to targets on computer displays. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 316–318.
  • Marks, L. E., & Algom, D. (1998). Psychophysical scaling. In M. H. Birnbaum (Eds.), Measurement, judgment, and decision making (pp. 81–178). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R. A., Kornblum, S., Wright, C. E., & Smith, J. E. (1988). Optimality in human motor performance: Ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychological Review, 95, 340–370.
  • Monlezun, C. J. (1999). Fitting polynomials to main effects in multi factor experiments: Unequal spacing and unbalanced data with no missing cells. Communications in Statistics-Theory and Methods, 28, 2899–2916.
  • Munro, H., Plumb, M. S., Wilson, A. D., Williams, J. H., & Mon-Williams, M. (2007). The effect of distance on reaction time in aiming movements. Experimental Brain Research, 183, 249–257.
  • Patla, A. E., Frank, J. S., Allard, F., & Thomas, E. (1985). Speed-accuracy characteristics of saccadic eye movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 17, 411–419.
  • Pedhazur, E. J. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction (3rd ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace College Publishers.
  • Plamondon, R., & Alimi, A. M. (1997). Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 279–303; discussion 303–349.
  • Robinson, D. A. (1964). The mechanics of human saccadic eye movement. Journal of Physiology, 174, 245–264.
  • Robinson, D. A. (1981). Control of eye movements. In Comprehensive physiology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. ( Original work published 1981)
  • Rosenbaum, D. A., & Patashnik, O. (1980). Time to time in the human motor system. In R. N. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 93–106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Schmidt, R. A., Zelaznik, H., Hawkins, B., Frank, J. S., & Quinn, J. T. (1979). Motor-output variability: A theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychological Review, 47, 415–451.
  • Sleimen-Malkoun, R., Temprado, J. J., Huys, R., Jirsa, V., & Berton, E. (2012). Is Fitts' law continuous in discrete aiming? PloS One, 7, e41190.
  • Temprado, J. J., Sleimen-Malkoun, R., Lemaire, P., Rey-Robert, B., Retornaz, F., & Berton, E. (2013). Aging of sensorimotor processes: A systematic study in Fitts' task. Experimental Brain Research, 228, 105–116.
  • Vergilino, D., & Beauvillain, C. (2001). Reference frames in reading: Evidence from visually and memory-guided saccades. Vision Research, 41, 3547–3557.
  • Weiler, J., & Heath, M. (2014). Oculomotor task switching: Alternating from a nonstandard to a standard response yields the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost. Journal of Neurophysiology, 112, 2176–2184.
  • Welford, A. T. (1968). Fundamentals of skill. Methuen's manuals of modern psychology. London: Methuen.
  • Wenban-Smith, M. G., & Findlay, J. M. (1991). Express saccades: Is there a separate population in humans? Experimental Brain Research, 87, 218–222.
  • Wu, C. C., Kwon, O. S., & Kowler, E. (2010). Fitts's law and speed/accuracy trade-offs during sequences of saccades: Implications for strategies of saccadic planning. Vision Research, 50, 2142–57.
  • Wurtz, R. H., & Albano, J. E. (1980). Visual-motor function of the primate superior colliculus. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 3, 189–226.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.