195
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Interaction of Perception and Action in Discrete and Continuous Rapid Aiming Tasks

, , &
Pages 524-532 | Received 25 Mar 2016, Accepted 31 Jul 2016, Published online: 29 Dec 2016

REFERENCES

  • Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 1109–1116.
  • Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–685.
  • Alphonsa, S., Dai, B., Benham-Deal, T., & Zhu, Q. (2016). Combined visual illusion effects on the perceived index of difficulty and movement outcomes in discrete and continuous Fitts’ tapping. Psychological Research, 80, 55–68.
  • Beets, I. A., Rosler, F., & Fiehler, K. (2010). Nonvisual motor learning improves visual motion perception: Evidence from violating the two-thirds power law. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104, 1612–1624. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00974.2009 [doi]
  • Cant, J. S., Westwood, D. A., Valyear, K. F., & Goodale, M. A. (2005). No evidence for visuomotor priming in a visually guided action task. Neuropsychologia, 43, 216–226.
  • Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16, 69–74.
  • Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Umiltà, C. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Evidence for visuomotor priming effect. Neuroreport, 8, 347–349.
  • Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381–391.
  • Fitts, P. M., & Peterson, J. R. (1964). Information capacity of discrete motor responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 103–112.
  • Franz, V. H., Fahle, M., Bülthoff, H. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2001). Effects of visual illusions on grasping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1124–1144.
  • Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bulthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11, 20–25.
  • Gentilucci, M., Chieffi, S., Daprati, E., Saetti, M. C., & Toni, I. (1996). Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia, 34, 369–376.
  • Glover, S. (2002). Visual illusions affect planning but not control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 288–292.
  • Haffenden, A. M., & Goodale, M. A. (1998). The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 122–136.
  • Hecht, H., Vogt, S., & Prinz, W. (2001). Motor learning enhances perceptual judgment: A case for action-perception transfer. Psychological Research, 65, 3–14.
  • Knol, H., Huys, R., Sarrazin, J. C., & Jirsa, V. K. (2015). Quantifying the Ebbinghaus figure effect: target size, context size, and target-context distance determine the presence and direction of the illusion. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1679.
  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1993). Visual pathways to perception and action. Progress in Brain Research, 95, 317–317.
  • Mou, W., & McNamara, T. P. (2002). Intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 162–170.
  • Schmidt, R. A., Zelaznik, H., Hawkins, B., Frank, J. S., & Quinn Jr, J. T. (1979). Motor-output variability: A theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychological Review, 86, 415–451.
  • van Donkelaar, P. (1999). Pointing movements are affected by size-contrast illusions. Experimental Brain Research, 125, 517–520.
  • Westwood, D. A., Chapman, C. D., & Roy, E. A. (2000). Pantomimed actions may be controlled by the ventral visual stream. Experimental Brain Research, 130, 545–548.
  • Westwood, D. A., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. Spatial Vision, 16, 243–254.
  • Wilson, A. D., Snapp-Childs, W., & Bingham, G. P. (2010). Perceptual learning immediately yields new stable motor coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1508–1514.
  • Zhu, Q. (2013). Perceiving the affordance of string tension for power strokes in badminton: Expertise allows effective use of all string tensions. Journal of Sports Sciences, 31, 1187–1196.
  • Zhu, Q., & Bingham, G. P. (2010). Learning to perceive the affordance for long-distance throwing: Smart mechanism or function learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 862–875.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.