212
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

Polar and Cartesian Structure in the Data of Fitts’s (1954) Classic Experiments—with a Criterion for Distinguishing a Strong and a Weak Version of Fitts’ Law

Pages 271-293 | Received 05 Dec 2018, Accepted 18 Apr 2019, Published online: 23 May 2019

References

  • Abrams, R. A., Meyer, D. E., & Kornblum, S. (1989). Speed and accuracy of saccadic eye movements: Characteristics of impulse variability in the oculomotor system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(3), 529. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.15.3.529
  • Accot, J., & Zhai, S. (2001). Scale effects in steering law tasks. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human factors in computing systems (pp. 1–8). ACM.
  • Akaike, H. (1973). Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In B. N. Petrov & F. Csáki (Eds.), Second International Symposium on Information Theory (pp. 267–281). Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.
  • Atkinson, D., & Peijnenburg, J. (2010). Crosswords and coherence. Review of Metaphysics, 63, 807–820.
  • Bongers, R., Fernandez, L., & Bootsma, R. J. (2009). Linear and logarithmic speed–accuracy trade-offs in reciprocal aiming result from task-specific parameterization of an invariant underlying dynamics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1443–1457. doi:10.1037/a0015783
  • Card, S. K., English, W. K., & Burr, B. J. (1978). Evaluation of mouse, rate-controlled isometric joystick, step keys, and text keys for text selection on a CRT. Ergonomics, 21(8), 601–613. doi:10.1080/00140137808931762
  • Crossman, E. R. F. W. (1956). The measurement of perceptual load in manual operations (Unpublished Ph D thesis). Birmingham University.
  • Crossman, E. R. F. W., & Goodeve, P. J. (1983). Feedback control of hand-movement and Fitts’ law. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(Pt 2), 251–278.
  • Elliott, D., Helsen, W. F., & Chua, R. (2001). A century later: Woodworth’s two-component model of goal-directed aiming. Psychological Bulletin, 127(3), 342–357. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.127.3.342
  • Fitts, P. M. (1953). The influence of response coding on performance in motor tasks. Current Trends in Information Theory. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 47–75.
  • Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(6), 381. Republished in 1992 in the Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 262–269. doi:10.1037/h0055392
  • Fitts, P. M., & Peterson, J. R. (1964). Information capacity of discrete motor responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(2), 103. doi:10.1037/h0045689
  • Flach, J. M., Guisinger, M. A., & Robison, A. B. (1996). Fitts’s Law: Nonlinear dynamics and positive entropy. Ecological Psychology, 8(4), 281–325. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco0804_1
  • Furnas, G. W., & Bederson, B. B. (1995). Space-scale diagrams: Understanding multiscale interfaces. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 234–241). New York: ACM Press/Addison-Wesley Publishing Co.
  • Gibbs, C. B. (1962). Controller design: Interactions of controlling limbs, time-lags, and gains in positional and velocity systems. Ergonomics, 5(2), 385–402. doi:10.1080/00140136208930602
  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Gori, J., Rioul, O., & Guiard, Y. (2018a). Speed-accuracy tradeoff: A formal information-theoretic transmission scheme (FITTS). ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 25(5), 1. doi:10.1145/3231595
  • Gori, J., Rioul, O., Guiard, Y., & Beaudouin-Lafon, M. (2018b). The Perils of confounding factors: How Fitts’ Law experiments can lead to false conclusions. In Proceedings of the 2018 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (p. 196). ACM.
  • Guiard, Y. (1997). Fitts’ law in the discrete vs. cyclical paradigm. Human Movement Science, 16(1), 97–131. doi:10.1016/S0167-9457(96)00045-0
  • Guiard, Y. (2009). The problem of consistency in the design of Fitts’ law experiments: Consider either target distance and width or movement form and scale. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 1809-1818). ACM. doi: 10.1145/1518701.1518980
  • Guiard, Y., & Olafsdottir, H. B. (2011). On the measurement of movement difficulty in the standard approach to Fitts’ law. PLoS One, 6(10), e24389. e24389.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024389
  • Guiard, Y., Olafsdottir, H. B., & Perrault, S. T. (2011). Fitts’ law as an explicit time/error trade-off. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 1619–1628). ACM.
  • Guiard, Y., & Rioul, O. (2015). A mathematical description of the speed/accuracy trade-off of aimed movement. In Proceedings of the 2015 British HCI Conference (pp. 91–100). ACM.
  • Haack, S. (1993/2009). Evidence and inquiry: A Pragmatist reconstruction of epistemology (2nd ed.). Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.
  • Harris, C. M., & Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature, 394(6695), 780–784. doi:10.1038/29528
  • Hess, R. A. (1973). Nonadjectival rating scales in human response experiments. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 13, 275–280. doi:10.1177/001872087301500311
  • Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4(1), 11–26. doi:10.1080/17470215208416600
  • Hoffmann, E. R. (2013). Which version/variation of Fitts’ law? A critique of information-theory models. Journal of Motor Behavior, 45(3), 205–215. doi:10.1080/00222895.2013.778815
  • ISO/DIS 9241-9 (2000). Ergonomic requirements for office work with visual display terminals (VDTs) - Part 9: Requirements for non-keyboard input devices. International Standard, International Organization for Standardization
  • Keele, S. W. (1968). Movement control in skilled motor performance. Psychological Bulletin, 70(6, Pt.1), 387–403. doi:10.1037/h0026739
  • Kelso, J. A. S. (1992). Theoretical concepts and strategies for understanding perceptual-motor skills: From information capacity in closed systems to self-organization in open, nonequilibrium systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(3), 260–261. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.121.3.260
  • Kuemmerle, W. (1998). Optimal scale for research and development in foreign environments: An investigation into size and performance of research and development laboratories abroad. Research Policy, 27(2), 111–126. doi:10.1016/S0048-7333(98)00030-4
  • Kvålseth, T. O. (1980). An alternative to Fitts’ law. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 16(5), 371–373. doi:10.3758/BF03329568
  • Luce, R. D. (1986). Response time distributions in memory search: A caution. In F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (Eds.), Mechanisms and performances (pp. 109–121). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
  • Luce, R. D. (2003). Whatever happened to information theory in psychology?. Review of General Psychology, 7(2), 183–188. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.7.2.183
  • MacKenzie, I. S. (1991). Fitts’ law as a performance model in human-computer interaction (Doctoral dissertation). University of Toronto. ISBN 0315659858
  • MacKenzie, I. S. (1992). Fitts’ law as a research and design tool in human-computer interaction. Human-Computer Interaction, 7(1), 91–139. doi:10.1207/s15327051hci0701_3
  • McMahon, T. A., & Bonner, J. T. (1983). On size and life. New York, NY: Scientific American Library.
  • Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R. A., Kornblum, S., Wright, C. E., & Smith, J. E. K. (1988). Optimality in human motor performance: Ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychological Review, 95(3), 340–370. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.3.340
  • Mottet, D., & Bootsma, R. J. (1999). The dynamics of goal-directed rhythmical aiming. Biological Cybernetics, 80(4), 235–245. doi:10.1007/s004220050521
  • Pachella, R. G. (1973). The interpretation of reaction time in information processing research (Technical report No. TR-45). Michigan University Ann Arbor Human Performance Center.
  • Pennycuick, C. J. (1992). Newton rules biology – A physical approach to biological problems. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Plamondon, R., & Alimi, A. M. (1997). Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 279–303.
  • Schmidt, R. A., Zelaznik, H. N., Hawkins, B., Frank, J. S., & Quinn, J. T. (1979). Motor-output variability: A theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychological Review, 86, 415–451.
  • Shannon, C. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27(3), 379–423. doi:10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb00917.x
  • Sheridan, M. R. (1979). A reappraisal of Fitts’ law. Journal of Motor Behavior, 11(3), 179–188. doi:10.1080/00222895.1979.10735186
  • Solomonoff, R. J. (1964). A formal theory of inductive inference. Information and Control, 7(1), 1–22.
  • Soukoreff, R. W., & MacKenzie, I. S. (2004). Towards a standard for pointing device evaluation, perspectives on 27 years of Fitts’ law research in HCI. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 61(6), 751–789. doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2004.09.001
  • Thompson, D. W. (1942/1992). On growth and form: A new edition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Viviani, P., & Terzuolo, C. (1982). Trajectory determines movement dynamics. Neuroscience, 7(2), 431–437. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(82)90277-9
  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1977). Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics. Acta Psychologica, 41(1), 67–85. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(77)90012-9
  • Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics: Control and communication in the animal and the machine. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Woodworth, R. S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychological Review, 3(3, Whole Suppl. 13), 1–119. doi:10.1037/h0092992
  • Wright, C. E., & Meyer, D. E. (1983). Conditions for a linear speed-accuracy trade-off in aimed movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 279–296. doi:10.1080/14640748308402134
  • Zelaznik, H. N., Mone, S., McCabe, G. P., & Thaman, C. (1988). Role of temporal and spatial precision in determining the nature of the speed/accuracy trade-off in aimed hand movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 221–230. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.14.2.221
  • Zelaznik, H. N., Shapiro, D. C., & McKolsky, D. (1981). Effect of a secondary task on the accuracy of single-aiming movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1007–1018. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.7.5.1007
  • Zhai, S. (2004). Characterizing computer input with Fitts’ law parameters—the information and non-information aspects of pointing. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 61(6), 791–809. doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2004.09.006

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.