118
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

Early Impulse Control: Treatment of Potential Errors within Pre-Programming and Control

&
Pages 713-722 | Received 23 May 2019, Accepted 10 Oct 2019, Published online: 04 Nov 2019

References

  • Beggs, W. D., & Howarth, C. I. (1972). The accuracy of aiming at a target. Some further evidence for a theory of intermittent control. Acta Psychologica, 36(3), 171–177. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(72)90001-7
  • Bennett, S. J., Elliott, D., & Rodacki, A. (2012). Movement strategies in vertical aiming in older adults. Experimental Brain Research, 216(3), 445–455. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2947-x
  • Carlton, L. G. (1992). Visual processing time and the control of movement. In L. Proteau & D. Elliott (Eds.), Vision and motor control (pp. 3–31). North-Holland: Elsevier.
  • Cluff, T., Crevecoeur, F., & Scott, S. H. (2015). A perspective on multisensory integration and rapid perturbation responses. Vision Research, 110(Pt B), 215–222. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.06.011
  • Cressman, E. K., Franks, I. M., Enns, J. T., & Chua, R. (2006). No automatic pilot for visually guided aiming based on colour. Experimental Brain Research, 717(2), 174–183. doi:10.1007/s00221-005-0260-2
  • Crossman, E. R. F. W., & Goodeve, P. J. (1983). Feedback control of hand-movement and Fitts' Law. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 35(Pt 2), 251–278.
  • Elliott, D., Dutoy, C., Andrew, M., Burkitt, J. J., Grierson, L. E. M., Lyons, J. L., … Bennett, S. J. (2014). The influence of visual feedback and prior knowledge about feedback on vertical aiming strategies. Journal of Motor Behavior, 46(6), 433–443. doi:10.1080/00222895.2014.933767
  • Elliott, D., Garson, R. G., Goodman, D., & Chua, R. (1991). Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming movements. Human Movement Science, 10(4), 393–418. doi:10.1016/0167-9457(91)90013-N
  • Elliott, D., Hansen, S., Grierson, L. E. M., Lyons, J., Bennett, S. J., & Hayes, S. J. (2010). Goal-directed aiming: Two components but multiple processes. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1023–1044. doi:10.1037/a0020958
  • Elliott, D., Hansen, S., Mendoza, J., & Tremblay, L. (2004). Learning to optimize speed, accuracy, and energy expenditure: A framework for understanding speed-accuracy relations in goal-directed aiming. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36(3), 339–351. doi:10.3200/JMBR.36.3.339-351
  • Elliott, D., Lyons, J., Hayes, S. J., Burkitt, J. J., Roberts, J. W., Grierson, L. E. M., … Bennett, S. J. (2017). The multiple process model of goal-directed reaching revisited. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 72, 95–110. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.016
  • Fradet, L., Lee, G., & Dounskaia, N. (2008). Origins of submovements during pointing movements. Acta Psychologica, 129(1), 91–100. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.04.009
  • Gomi, H. (2008). Implicit online corrections of reaching movements. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(6), 558–564. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2008.11.002
  • Gomi, H., Abekawa, N., & Nishida, S. (2006). Spatiotemporal tuning of rapid interactions between visual-motion analysis and reaching movement. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(20), 5301–5308. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0340-06.2006
  • Goodale, M. A., Pélisson, D., & Prablanc, C. (1986). Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature, 320(6064), 748–750. doi:10.1038/320748a0
  • Grierson, L. E. M., & Elliott, D. (2008). Kinematic analysis of goal-directed aims made against early and late perturbations: An investigation of the relative influence of two online control processes. Human Movement Science, 27(6), 839–856. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2008.06.001
  • Grierson, L. E. M., & Elliott, D. (2009a). Goal-directed aiming and the relative contribution of two online control processes. American Journal of Psychology, 122(3), 309–324.
  • Grierson, L. E. M., & Elliott, D. (2009b). The impact of real and illusory target perturbations on manual aiming. Experimental Brain Research, 193(3), 279–285. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1912-4
  • Grierson, L. E. M., Gonzalez, C., & Elliott, D. (2009). Kinematic analysis of early online control of goal-directed reaches: A novel movement perturbation study. Motor Control, 13(3), 280–296. doi:10.1123/mcj.13.3.280
  • Grierson, L. E. M., Lyons, J., & Elliott, D. (2011). The impact of real and illusory perturbations on the early trajectory adjustments of goal-directed movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43(5), 383–391. doi:10.1080/00222895.2011.606441
  • Hansen, S., Glazebrook, C., Anson, J. G., Weeks, D. J., & Elliott, D. (2006). The influence of advance information about target locationand visual feedback on movement planning and execution. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 60(3), 200–208. doi:10.1037/cjep2006019
  • Hansen, S., Tremblay, L., & Elliott, D. (2008). Real-time manipulation of visual displacement during manual aiming. Human Movement Science, 27(1), 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.09.001
  • Harris, C. M., & Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature, 394(6695), 780–784.
  • Keele, S. W., & Posner, M. I. (1968). Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77(1), 155–158. doi:10.1037/h0025754
  • Lyons, J., Hansen, S., Hurding, S., & Elliott, D. (2006). Optimizing rapid aiming behaviour: Movement kinematics depend on the cost of corrective modifications. Experimental Brain Research, 174(1), 95–100. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0426-6
  • Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R. A., Kornblum, S., Wright, C. E., & Smith, J. E. K. (1988). Optimality in human motor performance: Ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychological Review, 95(3), 340–370. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.3.340
  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442. doi:10.1163/156856897X00366
  • Proteau, L., & Masson, G. (1997). Visual perception modifies goal-directed movement control: Supporting evidence from a visual perturbation paradigm. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 50(4), 726–741. doi:10.1080/027249897391865
  • Proteau, L., Roujoula, A., & Messier, J. (2009). Evidence for continuous processing of visual information in a manual video-aiming task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41(3), 219–231. doi:10.3200/JMBR.41.3.219-231
  • Roberts, J. W., Burkitt, J. J., Elliott, D., & Lyons, J. L. (2016). The impact of strategic trajectory optimization on illusory target biases during goal-directed aiming. Journal of Motor Behavior, 48(6), 542–551. doi:10.1080/00222895.2016.1161588
  • Roberts, J., Burkitt, J. J., Willemse, B., Ludzki, A., Lyons, J., Elliott, D., & Grierson, L. E. M. (2013). The influence of target context and early and late vision on goal-directed reaching. Experimental Brain Research, 229(4), 525–532. doi:10.1007/s00221-013-3614-1
  • Smeets, J. B., & Brenner, E. (1995). Perception and action are based on the same visual information: Distinction between position and velocity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(1), 19–31. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.21.1.19
  • Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(69)90055-9
  • Tarita-Nistor, L., Gonzalez, E. G., Spigelman, A. J., & Steinbach, M. J. (2006). Linear vection as a function of stimulus eccentricity, visual angle, and fixation. Journal of Vestibular Research, 16(6), 265–272.
  • Tremblay, L., Hansen, S., Kennedy, A., & Cheng, D. T. (2013). The utility of vision during action: Multiple visuomotor processes. Journal of Motor Behavior, 45(2), 91–99. doi:10.1080/00222895.2012.747483
  • Von Holst, E. (1954). Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. The British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 2(3), 89–94. doi:10.1016/S0950-5601(54)80044-X
  • Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2005). The effects of response priming on the planning and execution of goal-directed movements in the presence of a distracting stimulus. Acta Psychologica, 119(2), 123–142. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.01.001
  • Whitney, D., Westwood, D. A., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object. Nature, 423(6942), 869–873. doi:10.1038/nature01693
  • Wolpert, D. M., & Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 3(S11), 1212–1217. doi:10.1038/81497
  • Wolpert, D. M., Miall, C. R., & Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(9), 338–347.
  • Woodworth, R. S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. The Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 3(3), 1–119. doi:10.1037/h0092992
  • Zelaznik, H. Z., Hawkins, B., & Kisselburgh, L. (1983). Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15(3), 217–236. doi:10.1080/00222895.1983.10735298
  • Zhang, Y., Brenner, E., Duysens, J., Verschueren, S., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2018). Postural responses to target jumps and background motion in a fast pointing task. Experimental Brain Research, 236(6), 1573–1581. doi:10.1007/s00221-018-5222-6

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.