Publication Cover
Educational Psychology
An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology
Volume 39, 2019 - Issue 2
313
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Analytic character learning in Chinese children: evidence from associative pseudocharacter learning paradigm

Pages 223-235 | Received 11 Dec 2017, Accepted 02 Sep 2018, Published online: 16 Nov 2018

References

  • Chan, L., & Nunes, T. (1998). Children’s understanding of the formal and functional characteristics of written Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 115–131. doi:10.1017/S0142716400010614
  • Chow, B. W.-Y. (2014). The differential roles of paired associate learning in Chinese and English word reading abilities in bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 27, 1657–1672. doi:10.1007/s11145-014-9514-3
  • Department of Education. (1998). Ba shi si nian chang yong yu ci diao cha bao gao shu [Investigation report on frequently used words in 1995]. Retrieved from http://language.moe.gov.tw/001/Upload/files/SITE_CONTENT/M0001/CDBOOK/C16.HTM
  • Ehri, L. C. (1999). Phases of development in learning to read words. In J. Oakhill & R. Beard (Eds.), Reading development and the teaching of reading: A psychological perspective (pp. 79–108). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Science.
  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neurological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301–330). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W., Tsang, S.-M., Lee, S.-H., & Chung, K. K.-H. (2006). Word learning deficit among Chinese dyslexic children. Journal of Child Language, 33(1), 145–161. doi:10.1017/S0305000905007154
  • Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., Lee, S.-H., Tsang, S.-M., & Luan, V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91, 43–75. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00163-X
  • Ho, C. S.-H., Yau, P. W.-Y., & Au, A. (2003). Development of orthographic knowledge and its relationship with reading and spelling among Chinese kindergarten and primary school children. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 51–71). Westport, CT: Praeger.
  • Hoosain, R. (1991). Psycholinguistic implications for linguistic relativity: A case study of Chinese. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Hsiao, J. H.-W., & Shillcock, R. (2006). Analysis of a Chinese phonetic compound database: Implications for orthographic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 405–426. doi:10.1007/s10936-006-9022-y
  • Huang, H. S. & Hanley, J. R. (1995). Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English. Cognition, 54, 73–98. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)00641-W
  • Hulme, C., Goetz, K., Gooch, D., Adams, J., & Snowling, M. J. (2007). Paired-associate learning, phoneme awareness, and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 150–166. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2006.09.002
  • Li, J., Fu, X. L. & Lin, Z. X. (2000). Study on the development of Chinese orthographic regularity in school children. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 32, 121–126. Retrieved from http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2000/V32/I02/121
  • Li, H., Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Liu, H. Y., & Xue, J. (2009). Paired associate learning in Chinese children with dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 135–151. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.001
  • Li, H., Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Liu, H., & Peng, H. (2012). Chinese children's character recognition: Visuo-orthographic, phonological processing and morphological skills. Journal of Research in Reading, 35, 287–307. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01460.x
  • Liu, P. D., Chung, K. K., McBride-Chang, C., & Tong, X. (2010). Holistic versus analytic processing: Evidence for a different approach to processing of Chinese at the word and character levels in Chinese children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 466–478. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.006
  • McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children's Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743–751. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.743
  • Messbauer, V. C. S., & de Jong, P. F. (2003). Word, nonword, and visual paired associate learning in Dutch dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 77–96. doi:10.1016/s0022-0965(02)00179-0
  • Mourgues, C., Tan, M., Hein, S., Ojanen, E., Reich, J., Lyytinen, H., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2016). Paired associate learning tasks and their contribution to reading skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 46, 54–63. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2014.12.003
  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665–681. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.665
  • Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. T. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189–213). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi:10.1075/swll.11.14per
  • Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568. doi:10.1037//0033-295x.96.4.523
  • Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1997). Role of radical awareness in the character and word acquisition of Chinese children. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 78–89. doi:10.1598/RRQ.32.1.5
  • Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child development, 74, 27–47. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00519
  • Wang, P. (2009). Shuo wen sheng fu yu han zi ji ben zi yan jiu [A study of phonetic elements and primary words of Chinese characters in Shuo wen]. The Journal of Chinese Characters, 1, 173–211.
  • Warmington, M., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phoneme awareness, visual-verbal paired-associate learning, and rapid automatized naming as predictors of individual differences in reading ability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 45–62. doi:10.1080/10888438.2010.534832
  • Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (1998). Poor reading: A deficit in skill-automatization or a phonological deficit? Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 321–340. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0204_2
  • Windfuhr, K. L., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The relationship between paired associate learning and phonological skills in normally developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 160–173. doi:10.1006/jecp.2000.2625
  • Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Spearing, D. (1995). Semantic and phonological coding in poor and normal readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 76–123. doi:10.1006/jecp.1995.1004
  • Vellutino, F. R., Steger, J. A., Harding, C. J., & Phillips, F. (1975). Verbal vs non-verbal paired-associates learning in poor and normal readers. Neuropsychologia, 13, 75–82. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(75)90050-0
  • Yin, L., & McBride, C. (2015). Chinese kindergartners learn to read characters analytically. Psychological Science, 26, 424–432. doi:10.1177/095679761456720

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.