1,194
Views
69
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Similar neural correlates for language and sequential learning: Evidence from event-related brain potentials

, &
Pages 231-256 | Received 01 Apr 2010, Accepted 09 Jun 2011, Published online: 20 Oct 2011

REFERENCES

  • Allen , M. , Badecker , W. and Osterhout , L. 2003 . Morphological analysis in sentence processing: An ERP study . Language & Cognitive Processes , 18 : 405 – 430 .
  • Altmann , G. T. M. and Dienes , Z. 1999 . Rule learning by seven-month-old infants and neural networks . Science , 284 : 875
  • Aslin , R. N. , Saffran , J. R. and Newport , E. L. 1998 . Computation of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants . Psychological Science , 9 : 321 – 324 .
  • Bahlmann , J. , Gunter , T. C. and Friederici , A. D. 2006 . Hierarchical and linear sequence processing: An electrophysiological exploration of two different grammar types . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 18 : 1829 – 1842 .
  • Baldwin , K. B. and Kutas , M. 1997 . An ERP analysis of implicit structured sequence learning . Psychophysiology , 34 : 74 – 86 .
  • Barber , H. and Carreiras , M. 2005 . Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 17 : 137 – 153 .
  • Barr , M. 2007 . The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 11 : 280 – 289 .
  • Bornkessel-Schlesewsky , I. and Schlesewsky , M. 2008 . An alternative perspective on “semantic P600” effects in language comprehension . Brain Research Reviews , 59 : 55 – 73 .
  • Brooks , P. J. , Braine , M. D. S. , Catalano , L. , Brody , R. E. and Sudhalter , V. 1993 . Acquisition of gender-like noun subclasses in an artificial language: The contribution of phonological markers to learning . Journal of Memory and Language , 32 : 76 – 95 .
  • Carrión , R. E. and Bly , B. M. 2007 . Event-related potential markers of expectation violation in an artificial grammar learning task . NeuroReport , 18 : 191 – 195 .
  • Chambers , K. E. , Onishi , K. H. and Fisher , C. 2003 . Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience . Cognition , 87 : B69 – B77 .
  • Christiansen , M. H. and Chater , N. 2008 . Language as shaped by the brain . Behavioral & Brain Sciences , 31 : 489 – 558 .
  • Christiansen , M. H. , Conway , C. M. , & Curtin , S. 2000 . Gleitman L. R. , & Joshi A. K. A connectionist single-mechanism account of rule-like behavior in infancy . The proceedings of the 22nd annual conference of the cognitive science society 83 – 88 Mahwah , NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum .
  • Christiansen , M. H. , Kelly , M. L. , Shillcock , R. C. and Greenfield , K. 2010 . Impaired artificial grammar learning in agrammatism . Cognition , 116 : 382 – 393 .
  • Cleeremans , A. , Destrebecqz , A. and Boyer , M. 1998 . Implicit learning: News from the front . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 2 : 406 – 416 .
  • Cleeremans , A. and McClelland , J. L. 1991 . Learning the structure of event sequences . Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 120 : 235 – 253 .
  • Clegg , B. A. , DiGirolamo , G. J. and Keele , S. W. 1998 . Sequence learning . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 2 : 275 – 281 .
  • Conway , C. M. , Bauernschmidt , A. , Huang , S. S. and Pisoni , D. B. 2010 . Implicit statistical learning in language processing: Word predictability is the key . Cognition , 114 : 356 – 371 .
  • Conway , C. M. and Christiansen , M. H. 2001 . Sequential learning in non-human primates . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 5 : 539 – 546 .
  • Conway , C. M. and Christiansen , M. H. 2005 . Modality-constrained statistical learning of tactile, visual, and auditory sequences . Journal of Experimental Psychology , 31 : 24 – 39 .
  • Conway , C. M. and Christiansen , M. H. 2006 . Statistical learning within and between modalities: Pitting abstract against stimulus specific representations . Psychological Science , 17 : 905 – 912 .
  • Conway , C. M. , Ellefson , M. R. , & Christiansen , M. H. 2003 . Alterman R. Kirsh D. When less is less and when less is more: Starting small with staged input . Proceedings of the 25th annual conference of the cognitive science society 270 – 275 Mahwah , NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum .
  • Conway , C. M. , Karpicke , J. and Pisoni , D. B. 2007 . Contribution of implicit sequence learning to spoken language processing: Some preliminary findings from normal-hearing adults . Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education , 12 : 317 – 334 .
  • Coulson , S. , King , J. W. and Kutas , M. 1998 . Expect the unexpected: Event-related brain response to morphosyntactic violations . Language and Cognitive Processes , 13 : 21 – 58 .
  • Coulson , S. and Kutas , M. 2001 . Getting it: Human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders . Neuroscience Letters , 316 : 71 – 74 .
  • Coulson , S. and Lovett , C. 2004 . Handedness, hemispheric asymmetry, and joke comprehension . Cognitive Brain Research , 19 : 275 – 288 .
  • Coulson , S. and Van Petten , C. 2002 . Conceptual integration and metaphor: An ERP Study . Memory & Cognition , 30 : 958 – 968 .
  • Coulson , S. and Van Petten , C. 2007 . A special role for the right hemisphere in metaphor comprehension: An ERP Study . Brain Research , 1146 : 128 – 145 .
  • Coulson , S. and Williams , R. F. 2005 . Hemispheric asymmetries and joke comprehension . Neuropsychologia , 43 : 128 – 141 .
  • Crocker , M. W. , Knoeferle , P. and Mayberry , M. R. 2010 . Situated sentence processing: The coordinated interplay account and a neurobehavioral model . Brain and Language , 112 : 189 – 201 .
  • Curran , T. and Keele , S. W. 1993 . Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning . Journal of Experimental Psychology , 19 : 189 – 202 .
  • Dell , G. S. , Reed , K. D. , Adams , D. R. and Meyer , A. S. 2000 . Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and implicit learning: A study of the role of experience in language production . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 26 : 1355 – 1367 .
  • Delorme , A. and Makeig , S. 2004 . EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics . Journal of Neuroscience Methods , 134 : 9 – 21 .
  • Eimer , M.,T. Goschke , T. , Schlaghecken , F. , & Stürmer , B. 1996 . Explicit and implicit learning of event sequences: Evidence from event-related brain potentials . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 22 , 970 – 987 .
  • Federmeier , K. 2007 . Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension . Psychophysiology , 44 : 491 – 505 .
  • Felser , C. , Clahsen , H. and Münte , T. F. 2003 . Storage and integration in the processing of filler-gap dependencies: An ERP study of topicalization and wh-movement in German . Brain and Language , 87 : 345 – 354 .
  • Ferdinand , N. K. , Mecklinger , A. and Kray , J. 2008 . Error deviance processing in implicit and explicit sequence learning . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 20 : 629 – 642 .
  • Ferguson , M. J. and Bargh , J. A. 2004 . How social perception can automatically influence behavior . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 8 : 33 – 39 .
  • Ferree , T. C. , Luu , P. L. , Russell , J. and Tucker , D. M. 2001 . Scalp electrode impedance, infection risk, and EEG data quality . Clinical Neurophysiology , 112 : 536 – 544 .
  • Fiebach , C. J. , Schlesewsky , M. and Friederici , A. D. 2002 . Separating syntactic memory costs and syntactic integration costs during parsing: The processing of German wh-questions . Journal of Memory and Language , 47 : 250 – 272 .
  • Fiser , J. and Aslin , R. N. 2002 . Statistical learning of higher-order temporal structure from visual shape sequences . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 28 : 458 – 467 .
  • Friederici , A. D. 1995 . The time course of syntactic activation during language processing: A model based on neuropsychological and neurophysiological data . Brain and Language , 50 : 259 – 281 .
  • Friederici , A. D. , Hahne , A. and Mecklinger , A. 1996 . Temporal structure of syntactic parsing: Early and late event-related brain potential effects . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 22 : 1219 – 1248 .
  • Friederici , A. D. , Pfeifer , E. and Hahne , A. 1993 . Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: Effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violations . Cognitive Brain Research , 1 : 183 – 192 .
  • Friederici , A. D. , Steinhauer , K. and Pfeifer , E. 2002 . Brain signatures of artificial language processing: Evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 99 : 529 – 534 .
  • Frigo , L. and McDonald , J. L. 1998 . Properties of phonological markers that affect the acquisition of gender-like subclasses . Journal of Memory and Language , 39 : 218 – 245 .
  • Gómez , R. L. 2002 . Variability and detection of invariant structure . Psychological Science , 13 : 431 – 436 .
  • Gómez , R. L. and Gerken , L. A. 2000 . Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 4 : 178 – 186 .
  • Gouvea , A. C. , Phillips , C. , Kazanina , N. and Poeppel , D. 2010 . The linguistic processes underlying the P600 . Language and Cognitive Processes , 25 : 149 – 188 .
  • Green , T. R. G. 1979 . Necessity of syntax markers: 2 experiments with artificial languages . Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , 18 : 481 – 496 .
  • Gunter , T. C. , Stowe , L. A. and Mulder , G. 1997 . When syntax meets semantics . Psychophysiology , 34 : 600 – 676 .
  • Hagoort , P. 2003 . How the brain solves the binding problem for language: A neurocomputational model of syntactic processing . NeuroImage , 20 : S18 – S29 .
  • Hagoort , P. 2009 . Reflections on the neurobiology of syntax . In D. Bickerton & E. Szathmáry Biological foundations and origin of syntax. Strüngmann Forum Reports 279 – 296 Cambridge , MA : 3 MIT Press .
  • Hagoort , P. , & Brown , C. M. 1994 . Brain responses to lexical ambiguity resolution and parsing . In C. Clifton Jr. , L. Frazier , & K. Rayner Perspectives on sentence processing (pp. 45 – 80 ). Hillsdale , NJ : Erlbaum .
  • Hagoort , P. , Brown , C. M. and Groothusen , J. 1993 . The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing . Language and Cognitive Processes , 8 : 439 – 484 .
  • Hagoort , P. , Brown , C. M. and Osterhout , L . 1999 . “ The neural architecture of syntactic processing ” . In Neurocognition of language , Edited by: Brown , C. M. and Hagoort , P. 273 – 316 . Oxford , , UK : Oxford University Press .
  • Hahne , A. 2001 . What's different in second-language processing? Evidence from event-related brain potentials . Journal of Psycholinguistic Research , 30 : 251 – 266 .
  • Heimbauer , L. A. , Conway , C. M. , Christiansen , M. H. , Beran , M. J. , & Owren , M. J. 2010 . Grammar rule-based sequence learning by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) . American Journal of Primatology , 72 65 .
  • Hoen , M. and Dominey , P. F. 2000 . ERP analysis of cognitive sequencing: A left anterior negativity related to structural transformation processing . Neuroreport , 11 : 3187 – 3191 .
  • Hoen , M. , Golembiowski , M. , Guyot , E. , Deprez , V. , Caplan , D. and Dominey , P. F. 2003 . Training with cognitive sequences improves syntactic comprehension in agrammatic aphasics . NeuroReport , 14 : 495 – 499 .
  • Hunt , R. H. and Aslin , R. N. 2001 . Statistical learning in a serial reaction time task: Access to separable statistical cues by individual learners . Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 130 : 658 – 680 .
  • Jentschke , S. and Koelsch , S. 2009 . Musical training modulates the development of syntax processing in children . NeuroImage , 47 : 735 – 744 .
  • Johnstone , T. and Shanks , D. R. 1999 . Two mechanisms in implicit artificial grammar learning? Comment on Meulemans and Van der Linden (1997) . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition , 25 : 524 – 531 .
  • Kaan , E. 2009 . “ Fundamental syntactic phenomena and their putative relation to the brain ” . In Biological foundations and origin of syntax: Strüngmann Forum Reports , Edited by: Bickerton , D. and Szathmáry , E. Vol. 3 , 117 – 133 . Cambridge , MA : MIT Press .
  • Kaan , E. , Harris , A. , Gibson , E. and Holcomb , P. 2000 . The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty . Language and Cognitive Processes , 15 : 159 – 201 .
  • Kaan , E. and Swaab , T. Y. 2003 . Electrophysiological evidence for serial sentence processing: A comparison between non-preferred and ungrammatical continuations . Cognitive Brain Research, 3 , 17 : 621 – 635 .
  • Kamide , Y. 2008 . Anticipatory processes in sentence processing . Language and Linguistic Compass , 2 : 647 – 670 .
  • Kazmerski , V. A. , Blasko , D. G. and Dessalegn , B. G. 2003 . ERP and behavioral evidence of individual differences in metaphor comprehension . Memory & Cognition , 31 : 673 – 689 .
  • Kim , A. and Osterhout , L. 2005 . The independence of combinatory semantic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials . Journal of Memory and Language , 52 : 205 – 255 .
  • King , J. W. and Kutas , M. 1995 . Who did what and when? Using word- and clause-related ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 7 : 378 – 397 .
  • Kluender , R. and Kutas , M. 1993 . Bridging the gap: Evidence from ERPs on the processing of unbound dependencies . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 5 : 196 – 214 .
  • Knowlton , B. J. and Squire , L. R. 1994 . The information acquired during artificial grammar learning . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 20 : 79 – 91 .
  • Kuperberg , G. R. 2007 . Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax . Brain Research , 1146 : 23 – 49 .
  • Kuperberg , G. R. , Kreher , D. A. , Sitnikova , T. , Caplan , D. N. and Holcomb , P. J. 2007 . The role of animacy and thematic relationships in processing active English sentences: Evidence from event-related potentials . Brain and Language , 100 : 223 – 237 .
  • Kuperberg , G. R. , Sitnikova , T. , Caplan , D. and Holcomb , P. J. 2003 . Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences . Cognitive Brain Research , 17 : 117 – 129 .
  • Kutas , M. and Hillyard , S. A. 1980 . Reading senseless sentences: Brain potential reflect semantic incongruity . Science , 207 : 203 – 205 .
  • Lau , E. , Stroud , C. , Plesch , S. and Phillips , C. 2006 . The role of structural prediction in rapid syntactic analysis . Brain and Language , 98 : 74 – 88 .
  • Lelekov , T. , Dominey , P. F. and Garcia-Larrea , L. 2000 . Dissociable ERP profiles for processing rules vs instances in a cognitive sequencing task . NeuroReport , 11 : 1 – 4 .
  • MacDonald , M. C. , Pearlmutter , N. J. and Seidenberg , M. S. 1994 . The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution . Psychological Review , 101 : 676 – 703 .
  • Marcus , G. F. , Vijayan , S. , Bandi Rao , S. and Vishton , P. M. 1999 . Rule learning by seven-month-old infants . Science , 283 : 77 – 80 .
  • Meulemans , T. and Van der Linden , M. 1997 . Associative chunk strength in artificial grammar learning . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 23 : 1007 – 1028 .
  • Misyak , J. B. , Christiansen , M. H. and Tomblin , J. B. 2010 . Sequential expectations: The role of prediction-based learning in language . Topics in Cognitive Science , 2 : 138 – 153 .
  • Miyawaki , K. , Sato , A. , Yasuda , A. , Kumano , H. and Kuboki , T. 2005 . Explicit knowledge and intention to learn in sequence learning: An event-related potential study . NeuroReport , 16 : 705 – 708 .
  • Morgan , J. L. , Meier , R. P. and Newport , E. L. 1987 . Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language . Cognitive Psychology , 19 : 498 – 550 .
  • Mueller , J. L. , Bahlmann , J. and Friederici , A. D. 2008 . The role of pause cues in language learning: The emergence of event-related potential related to sequence processing . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 20 : 892 – 905 .
  • Mueller , J. L. , Hahne , A. , Fujii , Y. and Friederici , A. D. 2005 . Native and nonnative speakers’ processing of a miniature version of Japanese as revealed by ERPs . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 17 : 1229 – 1244 .
  • Münte , T. F. , Matzke , M. and Johannes , S. 1997 . Brain activity associated with syntactic incongruencies in words and pseudo-words . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 93 : 318 – 329 .
  • Neri , P. , Luu , J. Y. and Levi , D. M. 2006 . Meaningful interactions can enhance visual discrimination of human agents . Nature Neuroscience , 9 : 1186 – 1192 .
  • Neville , H. , Nicol , J. , Barss , A. , Forster , K. I. and Garrett , M. I. 1991 . Syntactically-based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 3 : 151 – 165 .
  • Nevins , A. , Dillon , B. , Malhotra , S. and Phillips , C. 2007 . The role of feature-number and feature-type in processing Hindi verb agreement violations . Brain Research , 1164 : 81 – 94 .
  • Newport , E. L. and Aslin , R. N. 2004 . Learning at a distance I: Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies . Cognitive Psychology , 48 : 127 – 162 .
  • Nissen , M. J. and Bullemer , P. 1987 . Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures . Cognitive Psychology , 19 : 1 – 32 .
  • Niv , Y. and Schoenbaum , G. 2008 . Dialogues on prediction errors . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 12 : 265 – 272 .
  • Oldfield , R. C. 1971 . The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory . Neuropsychologia , 9 : 97 – 113 .
  • Onnis , L. , Christiansen , M. H. , Chater , N. , & Gómez , R. 2003 . Reduction of uncertainty in human sequential learning: Evidence from artificial grammar learning . Alterman R. & Kirsh D. , Proceedings of the 25th annual conference of the cognitive science society 886 – 891 . Mahwah , NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum .
  • Onnis , L. , Farmer , T. A. , Baroni , M. , Christiansen , M. H. and Spivey , M. 2008 . Generalizable distributional regularities aid fluent language processing: The case of semantic valence tendencies . Italian Journal of Linguistics , 20 : 125 – 152 .
  • Onnis , L. , Waterfall , H. and Edelman , S. 2008 . Learn locally, act globally: Learning language from variation set cues . Cognition , 109 : 423 – 430 .
  • Osterhout , L. and Hagoort , P. 1999 . A superficial resemblance does not necessarily mean you are part of the family: Counterarguments to Coulson, King, & Kutas (1998) in the P600/SPS–P300 debate . Language and Cognitive Processes , 14 : 1 – 14 .
  • Osterhout , L. and Holcomb , P. J. 1992 . Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly . Journal of Memory and Language , 31 : 785 – 806 .
  • Osterhout , L. , Holcomb , P. J. and Swinney , D. A. 1994 . Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition , 20 : 786 – 803 .
  • Osterhout , L. and Mobley , L. A. 1995 . Event-related potentials elicited by failure to agree . Journal of Memory and Language , 34 : 739 – 773 .
  • Pacton , S. , Perruchet , P. , Fayol , M. and Cleeremans , A. 2001 . Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities . Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 130 : 401 – 426 .
  • Patel , A. D. , Gibson , E. , Ratner , J. , Besson , M. and Holcomb , P. J. 1998 . Processing syntactic relations in language and music: An event-related potential study . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 10 : 717 – 733 .
  • Peña , M. , Bonnatti , L. , Nespor , M. and Mehler , J. 2002 . Signal-driven computations in speech processing . Science , 298 : 604 – 607 .
  • Perruchet , P. and Pacton , S. 2006 . Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 10 : 233 – 238 .
  • Phillips , C. , Kazanina , N. and Abada , S. 2005 . ERP effects of the processing of syntactic long-distance dependencies . Cognitive Brain Research , 22 : 407 – 428 .
  • Pickering , M. J. and Garrod , S. 2007 . Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 11 : 105 – 110 .
  • Plante , E. , Gómez , R. and Gerken , L. 2002 . Sensitivity to word order cues by normal and language/learning disabled adults . Journal of Communication Disorders , 35 : 453 – 462 .
  • Pothos , E. M. 2007 . Theories of artificial grammar learning . Psychological Bulletin , 133 : 227 – 244 .
  • Pothos , E. M. and Bailey , T. M. 2000 . The role of similarity in artificial grammar learning . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 26 : 847 – 862 .
  • Rayner , K. and Well , A. D. 1999 . Effects of contextual constraint on eye movements in reading: A further examination . Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , 3 : 504 – 509 .
  • Reber , A. S. 1967 . Implicit learning of artificial grammars . Journal of Verbal Learning and Behavior , 6 : 855 – 863 .
  • Reed , J. and Johnson , P. 1994 . Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 20 : 585 – 594 .
  • Redington , M. and Chater , N. 1996 . Transfer in artificial grammar learning: A reevaluation . Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 125 : 123 – 138 .
  • Regel , S. , Coulson , S. and Gunter , T. C. 2010 . The communicative style of a speaker can affect language comprehension? ERP evidence from the comprehension of irony . Brain Research , 1311 : 121 – 135 .
  • Regel , S. , Gunter , T. C. and Friederici , A. D. 2011 . Isn't it ironic? An electrophysiological exploration of figurative language processing . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 23 : 277 – 293 .
  • Rossi , S. , Gugler , M. F. , Hahne , A. and Friederici , A. D. 2005 . When word category information encounters morphosyntax: An ERP study . Neuroscience Letters , 384 : 228 – 233 .
  • Rüsseler , J. , Hennighausen , E. , Münte , T. F. and Rösler , F. 2003 . Differences in incidental and intentional learning of sensorimotor sequences as revealed by event-related brain potentials . Cognitive Brain Research , 15 : 116 – 126 .
  • Rüsseler , J. , Hennighausen , E. and Rösler , F. 2001 . Response anticipation processes in the learning of a sensorimotor sequence: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential . Journal of Psychophysiology , 15 : 95 – 105 .
  • Rüsseler , J. and Rösler , F. 1999 . “ Representation and learning of structure in perceptuo-motor event-sequences ” . In Learning: Rule extraction and representation , Edited by: Friederici , A. and Menzel , R. 117 – 138 . New York : Walter de Gruyter .
  • Rüsseler , J. and Rösler , F. 2000 . Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: Evidence for distinct coding of perceptual and motor representations . Acta Psychologica , 104 : 45 – 67 .
  • Saffran , J. R. 2001 . Words in a sea of sounds: The output of infant statistical learning . Cognition , 81 : 149 – 169 .
  • Saffran , J. R. 2002 . Constraints on statistical language learning . Journal of Memory and Language , 47 : 172 – 196 .
  • Saffran , J. R. 2003 . Statistical learning: Mechanisms and constraints . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 12 : 110 – 114 .
  • Saffran , J. R. , Newport , E. L. , Aslin , R. N. , Tunick , R. A. and Barrueco , S. 1997 . Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear . Psychological Science , 8 : 101 – 105 .
  • Saffran , J. R. , Aslin , R. N. and Newport , E. L. 1996 . Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants . Science , 274 : 1926 – 1928 .
  • Saffran , J. R. , Johnson , E. K. , Aslin , R. N. and Newport , E. L. 1999 . Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults . Cognition , 70 : 27 – 52 .
  • Schlaghecken , F. , Stürmer , B. and Eimer , M. 2000 . Chunking processes in the learning of event sequences: Electrophysiological indicators . Memory & Cognition , 28 : 821 – 831 .
  • Seidenberg , M. S. and Elman , J. L. 1999 . Networks are not ‘hidden rules’ . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 3 : 288 – 289 .
  • Silva-Pereyra , J. , Conboy , B. T. , Klarman , L. and Kuhl , P. K. 2007 . Grammatical processing without semantics? An event-related brain potential study of preschoolers using jabberwocky sentences . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 19 : 1050 – 1065 .
  • Stadler , M. A. 1992 . Statistical structure and implicit serial learning . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory Cognition , 18 : 318 – 327 .
  • Steinhauer , K. and Connolly , J. F. 2008 . “ Event-related potentials in the study of language ” . In Handbook of the neuroscience of language , Edited by: Stemmer , B. and Whitaker , H. A. 91 – 104 . New York : Elsevier .
  • Tanenhaus , M. K. and Trueswell , J. C. 1995 . “ Sentence comprehension ” . In Speech, language, and communication , Edited by: Miller , J. and Eimas , P. 217 – 262 . San Diego , CA : Academic Press .
  • Tomblin , J. B. , Mainela-Arnold , E. and Zhang , X. 2007 . Procedural learning in adolescents with and without specific language impairment . Language Learning and Development , 3 : 269 – 293 .
  • Toro , J. M. , Sinnett , S. and Soto-Faraco , S. 2005 . Speech segmentation by statistical learning depends on attention . Cognition , 97 : B25 – B34 .
  • Tucker , D. M. 1993 . Spatial sampling of head electrical fields: The geodesic sensor net . Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology , 87 : 145 – 163 .
  • Turk-Browne , N. B. , Junge , J. A. and Scholl , B. J. 2005 . The automaticity of visual statistical learning . Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 134 : 522 – 564 .
  • Valian , V. and Coulson , S. 1988 . Anchor points in language learning: The role of marker frequency . Journal of Memory and Language , 27 : 71 – 86 .
  • Van Berkum , J. J. A. 2008 . Understanding sentences in context: What brain waves can tell us . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 17 : 376 – 380 .
  • Vokey , J. R. and Brooks , L. R. 1992 . Salience of item knowledge in learning artificial grammar . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 20 : 328 – 344 .
  • Wassenaar , M. , Brown , C. M. and Hagoort , P. 2005 . ERP effects of subject–verb agreement violations in patients with Broca's aphasia . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 16 : 553 – 576 .
  • Wassenaar , M. and Hagoort , P. 2005 . Word-category violations in patients with Broca's aphasia: An ERP study . Brain and Language , 92 : 117 – 137 .
  • Weber-Fox , C. M. and Neville , H. J. 1996 . Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 8 : 231 – 256 .
  • Ye , Z. , Luo , Y.-J. , Friederici , A. D. and Zhou , X. 2006 . Semantic and syntactic processing in Chinese sentence comprehension: Evidence from event-related potentials . Brain Research , 1071 : 186 – 196 .

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.