1,103
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

MEG evidence that the LIFG effect of object extraction requires similarity-based interference

&
Pages 381-389 | Received 11 Apr 2013, Accepted 29 Oct 2013, Published online: 05 Dec 2013

References

  • Adachi, Y., Shimogawara, M., Higuchi, M., Haruta, Y., & Ochiai, M. (2001). Reduction of non-periodic environmental magnetic noise in MEG measurement by continuously adjusted least squares method. IEEE Transactions on Applied Superconductivity, 11(1), 669–672. doi:10.1109/77.919433
  • Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2011). Simple composition: A magnetoencephalography investigation into the comprehension of minimal linguistic phrases. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 2801–2814. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5003-10.2011
  • Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2012). Basic Linguistic Composition Recruits the Left Anterior Temporal Lobe and Left Angular Gyrus During Both Listening and Reading. Cerebral Cortex, bhs170. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs170
  • Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Combination across domains: an MEG investigation into the relationship between mathematical, pictorial, and linguistic processing. Frontiers in Language Sciences, 3, 583. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00583
  • Ben-Shachar, M., Hendler, T., Kahn, I., Ben-Bashat, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2003). The neural reality of syntactic transformations: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychological Science, 14, 433–440. doi:10.2307/40064164
  • Ben-Shachar, M., Palti, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2004). Neural correlates of syntactic movement: Converging evidence from two fMRI experiments. NeuroImage, 21, 1320–1336. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.027
  • Berndt, R. S., & Caramazza, A. (1980). A redefinition of the syndrome of Broca's Aphasia: Implications for a neurological model of language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 225–278. doi:10.1017/S0142716400000552
  • Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Friederici, A. D., von Cramon, D. Y., & Schlesewsky, M. (2005). Who did what to whom? The neural basis of argument hierarchies during language comprehension. NeuroImage, 26, 221–233. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.032
  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
  • Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Waters, G. (1999). PET studies of syntactic processing with auditory sentence presentation. NeuroImage, 9, 343–351. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0412
  • Caplan, D., Alpert, N., Waters, G., & Olivieri, A. (2000). Activation of Broca's area by syntactic processing under conditions of concurrent articulation. Human Brain Mapping, 9(2), 65. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200002)9:2<65::AID-HBM1>3.3.CO;2-4
  • Caplan, D., Stanczak, L., & Waters, G. (2008). Syntactic and thematic constraint effects on blood oxygenation level dependent signal correlates of comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 643–656. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20044
  • Constable, R. T., Pugh, K. R., Berroya, E., Mencl, W. E., Westerveld, M., Ni, W., & Shankweiler, D. (2004). Sentence complexity and input modality effects in sentence comprehension: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 22(1), 11–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.001
  • Cooke, A., Zurif, E. B., DeVita, C., Alsop, D., Koenig, P., Detre, J., & Grossman, M. (2002). Neural basis for sentence comprehension: Grammatical and short-term memory components. Human Brain Mapping, 15(2), 80–94. doi:10.1002/hbm.10006
  • Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1989). Lesion analysis in neuropsychology. Oxford University Press.
  • Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Syntactic working memory and the establishment of filler-gap dependencies: Insights from ERPs and fMRI. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 321–338. doi:10.1023/A:1010447102554
  • Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., Lohmann, G., von Cramon, D. Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Revisiting the role of Broca's area in sentence processing: Syntactic integration versus syntactic working memory. Human Brain Mapping, 24(2), 79–91. doi:10.1002/hbm.20070
  • Ford, M. (1983). A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity throughout sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 203–218. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90156-1
  • Friederici, A. D., Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., Bornkessel, I. D., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Processing linguistic complexity and grammaticality in the left frontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1709–1717. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj106
  • Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 1411–1423. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1411
  • Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., Johnson, M., & Lee, Y. (2006). Similarity-based interference during language comprehension: Evidence from eye tracking during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1304–1321. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1304
  • Grewe, T., Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Wiese, R., Von Cramon, D. Y., & Schlesewsky, M. (2005). The emergence of the unmarked: A new perspective on the language-specific function of Broca's area. Human Brain Mapping, 26, 178–190. doi:10.1002/hbm.20154
  • Grodzinsky, Y. (1986). Language deficits and the theory of syntax. Brain and Language, 27(1), 135–159. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(86)90009-X
  • Grodzinsky, Y. (2000). The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca's area. Target article with 36 commentaries. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 47–117. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00502398
  • Grodzinsky, Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Neuroimaging of syntax and syntactic processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 240–246. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.007
  • Grodzinsky, Y., & Santi, A. (2008). The battle for Broca's region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 474–480. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.001
  • Hagoort, P. (2003). How the brain solves the binding problem for language: A neurocomputational model of syntactic processing. NeuroImage, 20(Suppl. 1), S18–S29. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.013
  • Hagoort, P. (2005). On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 416–423. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004
  • Hakes, D. T., Evans, J. S., & Brannon, L. L. (1976). Understanding sentences with relative clauses. Memory & Cognition, 4, 283–290. doi:10.3758/BF03213177
  • Holmes, V. M. (1973). Order of main and subordinate clauses in sentence perception. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 285–293. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80072-6
  • Holmes, V. M., & O'Regan, J. K. (1981). Eye fixation patterns during the reading of relative-clause sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 417–430. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90533-8
  • Huettel, S., Song, A., & Mccarthy, G. (2004). Functional magnetic resonance imaging [Sinauer Associates]. Retrieved from http://www.amazon.ca/exec/obidos/redirect?tag=citeulike09-20&path=ASIN/0878932887
  • Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., & Thulborn, K. R. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274, 114–116. doi:10.1126/science.274.5284.114
  • Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 159–201. doi:10.1080/016909600386084
  • Kaan, E., & Swaab, T. Y. (2002). The brain circuitry of syntactic comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 350–356. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01947-2
  • Keller, T. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2001). The neural bases of sentence comprehension: A fMRI examination of syntactic and lexical processing. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 223–237. doi:10.1093/cercor/11.3.223
  • King, J., & Just, M. A. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 580–602. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(91)90027-H
  • King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? Using word- and clause-level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 376–395. doi:10.1162/jocn.1995.7.3.376
  • Lancaster, J. L., Woldorff, M. G., Parsons, L. M., Liotti, M., Freitas, C. S., Rainey, L., … Fox, P. T. (2000). Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Human Brain Mapping, 10(3), 120–131. doi:10.1002/1097-0193(200007)10:3<120::AID-HBM30>3.0.CO;2-8
  • Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164, 177–190. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024
  • Maruyama, M., Pallier, C., Jobert, A., Sigman, M., & Dehaene, S. (2012). The cortical representation of simple mathematical expressions. NeuroImage, 16, 1444–1460. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.020
  • Mchombo, S. A. (Ed.). (1993). Theoretical aspects of Bantu grammar. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
  • Mosher, J. C., & Leahy, R. M. (1998). Recursive MUSIC: A framework for EEG and MEG source localization. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 45, 1342–1354. doi:10.1109/10.725331
  • Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2005). Cognitive control and parsing: Reexamining the role of Broca's area in sentence comprehension. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 263–281. doi:10.3758/CABN.5.3.263
  • Öztekin, I., Curtis, C. E., & McElree, B. (2009). The medial temporal lobe and the left inferior prefrontal cortex jointly support interference resolution in verbal working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1967–1979. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.21146
  • Öztekin, I., McElree, B., Staresina, B. P., & Davachi, L. (2008). Working memory retrieval: Contributions of the left prefrontal cortex, the left posterior parietal cortex, and the hippocampus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 581–593. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.21016
  • Penolazzi, B., Vincenzi, M. D., Angrilli, A., & Job, R. (2005). Processing of temporary syntactic ambiguity in Italian “who”-questions: A study with event-related potentials. Neuroscience Letters, 377, 91–96. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2004.11.074
  • Pylkkänen, L. (2008). Introducing arguments. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Rogalsky, C., Matchin, W., & Hickok, G. (2008). Broca's area, sentence comprehension, and working memory: An fMRI study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, 14. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.014.2008
  • Santi, A., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2007). Working memory and syntax interact in Broca's area. NeuroImage, 37(1), 8–17. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.047
  • Santi, A., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2012). Broca's area and sentence comprehension: A relationship parasitic on dependency, displacement or predictability? Neuropsychologia, 50, 821–832. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.017
  • Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Rauch, S. (1996). Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Language, 52, 452–473. doi:10.1006/brln.1996.0024
  • Thothathiri, M., Kim, A., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2012). Parametric effects of syntactic-semantic conflict in Broca's area during sentence processing. Brain and Language, 120, 259–264. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.004
  • Van Dyke, J. A., & McElree, B. (2006). Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 157–166. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.007
  • Vartiainen, J., Liljeström, M., Koskinen, M., Renvall, H., & Salmelin, R. (2011). Functional magnetic resonance imaging blood oxygenation level-dependent signal and magnetoencephalography evoked responses yield different neural functionality in reading. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31, 1048–1058. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3113-10.2011
  • Wanner, E., & Maratsos, M. (1978). An ATN approach to comprehension. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan, & G. Miller (Eds.), Linguistic Theory and Psychological Reality (pp. 119–161.).
  • Warren, T., & Gibson, E. (2005). Effects of NP type in reading cleft sentences in English. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20, 751–767. doi:10.1080/01690960500051055
  • Waters, G., Caplan, D., & Hildebrandt, N. (1987). Working memory and written sentence comprehension. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance 12: The psychology of reading (pp. 531–555). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Zurif, E. B. (1995). Brain regions of relevance to syntactic processing. In L. Gleitman & M. Liberman (Eds.), Invitation to Cognitive Sciences, Vol. I, 2nd edition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.