345
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Behavioural and Social Sciences

Directional aiming bias in golf putting

, &
Pages 364-369 | Accepted 15 May 2018, Published online: 03 Aug 2018

References

  • Bruno, N., & Franz, V. H. (2009). When is grasping affected by the Müller–Lyer illusion? A quantitative review. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1421–1433.
  • Calvin, W. H. (1982). Did throwing stones shape hominid brain evolution. Ethology and Sociobiology, 3(3), 115–124.
  • Cutting, J., & Vishton, P. M. (1995). Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth. In W. Epstein & S. Rogers (Eds.), perception of space and motion. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Gentilucci, M., Daprati, E., Gangitano, M., & Toni, I. (1997). Eye position tunes the contribution of allocentric and egocentric information to target localization in human goal-directed arm movements. Neuroscience Letters, 222, 123–126. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(97)13366-3
  • Hecht, H., Vogt, S., & Prinz, W. (2001). Motor learning enhances perceptual judgment: A case for action-perception transfer. Psychological Research, 65, 3–14.
  • Johnston, A., Benton, C. P., & Nishida, S. (2003). Last but not least - Golfers may have to overcome a persistent visuospatial illusion. Perception, 32(9), 1151–1154.
  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two-visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46, 774–785.
  • Previc, F. H. (1993). Functional interactions in 3-D visual space: Implications for visual displays. Society for Information Display International Symposium Digest of Technical Papers (Vol. 24, pp. 188–190). Playa del Rey, CA: Society for Information Display.
  • Shim, J., Miller, G., & Lutz, R. (2012). Use of ball line as an alignment aid in putting. International Journal of Golf Science, 1, 71–80.
  • Shim, J., & van der Kamp, J. (2017). The effects of optical illusions in perception and action in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Perception, 46(9), 1118–1126.
  • Shim, J., van der Kamp, J., Rigby, B. R., Lutz, R., Poolton, J. M., & Masters, R. S. W. (2014). Taking aim at the Müller-Lyer goalkeeper illusion: An illusion bias in action that originates from the target not being optically specified. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 40(3), 1274–1281.
  • Van der Kamp, J., de Wit, M., & Masters, R. S. W. (2012). Left, right, left, right, eyes to the front! Müller-Lyer bias in grasping is not a function of hand used, hand preferred or visual hemifield, but foveation does matter. Experimental Brain Research, 218(1), 91–98.
  • Van der Kamp, J., & Masters, R. S. W. (2008). The human Muller-Lyer illusion in goalkeeping. Perception, 37(6), 951–954.
  • Van Lier, W., van der Kamp, J., van der Zanden, A., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2011). No transfer of calibration between action and perception in learning a golf putting task. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 73(7), 2298–2308.
  • Van Lier, W. H., van der Kamp, J., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2011). Perception action in golf putting: Skill differences reflect calibration. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33(3), 349–369.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.