461
Views
16
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Localizing the deficit in a case of jargonaphasia

, &
Pages 211-238 | Received 30 Mar 2006, Accepted 07 Nov 2006, Published online: 10 Apr 2007

References

  • Aichert , I. and Ziegler , W. 2004 . Syllable frequency and syllable structure in apraxia of speech . Brain and Language , 88 : 148 – 159 .
  • Akaike , H. 1973 . “ Information theory as an extension of the maximum likelihood principle ” . In Second International Symposium on Information Theory , Edited by: Petrov , B. N. and Csaki , F. 267 – 281 . Budapest : Akademiai Kiado .
  • Alajouanine , T. 1956 . Verbal realization in aphasia . Brain , 79 : 22
  • Anderson , S. R. 1981 . Why phonology isn't “natural” . Linguistic Inquiry , 12 : 493 – 539 .
  • Anderson , S. R. and Lightfoot , D. W. 2002 . The language organ , Cambridge, , UK : Cambridge University Press .
  • Baayen , R. H. , Piepenbrock , R. and Van Rijn , H. 1995 . “ The CELEX Lexical Database ” . Philadelphia : University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium . [CD-ROM]
  • Baddeley , A. 1990 . Human memory: Theory and practice , Hove, , UK : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd .
  • Barry , C. , Hirsh , K. W. , Johnston , R. A. and Williams , C. L. 2001 . Age of acquisition, word frequency, and the locus of repetition priming of picture naming . Journal of Memory and Language , 44 : 350 – 375 .
  • Blumstein , S. E. 1973 . A phonological investigation of aphasic speech , The Hague, , The Netherlands : Mouton .
  • Brown , R. and McNeill , D. 1966 . The “tip of the tongue” phenomenon . Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , 5 : 325 – 337 .
  • Bub , D. , Black , S. , Howell , J. and Kertesz , A. 1987 . “ Speech output processes and reading ” . In The cognitive neuropsychology of language , Edited by: Coltheart , M. , Sartori , G. and Job , R. Hove, , UK : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd .
  • Buckingham , H. W. 1977 . Conduction theory and neologistic jargon . Language and Speech , 20 : 174 – 184 .
  • Buckingham , H. W. 1981 . “ Where do neologisms come from? ” . In Jargonaphasia , Edited by: Brown , J. New York : Academic Press .
  • Buckingham , H. W. and Kertesz , A. 1976 . Neologistic jargon aphasia , Amsterdam : Swets and Zeitlinger .
  • Burnham , K. P. and Anderson , D. R. 2002 . Model selection and multimodel inference: A practical information-theoretic approach , (2nd ed.). , New York : Springer .
  • Burzio , L. 2002 . “ Surface-to-surface morphology: When your representations turn into constraints ” . In Many morphologies , Edited by: Boucher , P. Somerville, MA : Cascadilla Press .
  • Butterworth , B. 1979 . Hesitation and the production of verbal paraphasias and neologisms in jargon aphasia . Brain & Language , 8 : 133 – 161 .
  • Caramazza , A. 1997 . How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 14 : 177 – 208 .
  • Caramazza , A. , Miceli , G. and Villa , G. 1986 . The role of the (output) phonological buffer in reading, writing, and repetition . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 3 : 37 – 76 .
  • Caramazza , A. and Miozzo , M. 1997 . The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: Evidence from the “tip-of-the-tongue” phenomenon . Cognition , 64 : 309 – 343 .
  • Chomsky , N. and Halle , M. 1968 . The sound pattern of English , MA: Cambridge : The MIT Press .
  • Clements , G. N. 1990 . “ The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification ” . In Papers in laboratory phonology 1 , Edited by: Kingston , J. and Beckman , M. E. Cambridge, , UK : Cambridge University Press .
  • Cohen , L. and Dehaene , S. 1998 . Competition between past and present—assessment and interpretation of verbal perseverations . Brain , 121 : 1641 – 1659 .
  • Cohen , L. , Verstichel , P. and Dehaene , S. 1997 . Neologistic jargon sparing numbers: A category-specific phonological impairment . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 14 : 1029 – 1061 .
  • Coltheart , M. , Rastle , K. , Perry , C. , Langdon , R. and Ziegler , J. 2001 . DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud . Psychological Review , 108 : 204 – 256 .
  • Cowan , G. 1998 . Statistical data analysis , Oxford, , UK : Clarendon Press .
  • Davies , P. D. and Funnell , E. 2000 . Semantic representation and ease of predication . Brain and Language , 73 : 92 – 119 .
  • Dell , G. S. 1986 . A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production . Psychological Review , 93 : 283 – 321 .
  • Dell , G. S. 1988 . The retrieval of phonological forms in production—tests of predictions from a connectionist model . Journal of Memory and Language , 27 : 124 – 142 .
  • Dell , G. S. and O'Seaghdha , P. G. 1992 . Stages of lexical access in language production . Cognition , 42 : 287 – 314 .
  • Forde , E. M. E. and Humphreys , G. W. 1997 . A semantic locus for refractory behaviour: Implications for access-storage distinctions and the nature of semantic memory . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 14 : 367 – 402 .
  • Goldrick , M. and Rapp , B. 2007 . Lexical and post-lexical phonological representations in spoken production . Cognition , 102 ( 2 ) : 219 – 260 .
  • Goldsmith , J. A. 1990 . Autosegmental and metrical phonology , Oxford, , UK : Basil Blackwell .
  • Goodglass , H. , Kaplan , E. and Weintraub , S. 1983 . The Boston Naming Test , Philadelphia : Lea & Febiger .
  • Graham , N. L. , Patterson , K. and Hodges , J. R. 2001 . The emergence of jargon in progressive fluent dysgraphia: The widening gap between target and response . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 18 : 343 – 361 .
  • Gusfield , D. 1997 . Algorithms on strings, trees, and sequences: Computer science and computational biology , Cambridge, , UK : Cambridge University Press .
  • Hanley , J. R. , Dell , G. S. , Kay , J. and Baron , R. 2004 . Evidence for the involvement of a nonlexical route in the repetition of familiar words: A comparison of single and dual route models of auditory repetition . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 21 : 147 – 158 .
  • Hanley , J. R. and Kay , J. 1997 . An effect of imageability on the production of phonological errors in auditory repetition . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 14 : 1065 – 1084 .
  • Hanley , J. R. , Kay , J. and Edwards , M. 2002 . Imageability effects, phonological errors, and the relationship between auditory repetition and picture naming: Implications for models of auditory repetition . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 19 : 193 – 206 .
  • Harley , T. A. and Bown , H. E. 1998 . What causes a tip-of-the-tongue state? Evidence for lexical neighbourhood effects in speech production . British Journal of Psychology , 89 : 151 – 174 .
  • Harm , M. W. and Seidenberg , M. S. 2004 . Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes . Psychological Review , 111 : 662 – 720 .
  • Hillis , A. E. , Boatman , D. , Hart , J. and Gordon , B. 1999 . Making sense out of jargon—a neurolinguistic and computational account of jargon aphasia . Neurology , 53 : 1813 – 1824 .
  • Hillis , A. E. and Caramazza , A. 1991 . Mechanisms for accessing lexical representations for output—evidence from a category-specific semantic deficit . Brain and Language , 40 : 106 – 144 .
  • Hillis , A. E. and Caramazza , A. 1995 . Converging evidence for the interaction of semantic and sublexical phonological information in accessing lexical representations for spoken output . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 12 : 187 – 227 .
  • Jescheniak , J. D. and Levelt , W. J. M. 1994 . Word-frequency effects in speech production—retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition , 20 : 824 – 843 .
  • Jones , G. V. 1985 . Deep dyslexia, imageability, and ease of predication . Brain and Language , 24 : 1 – 19 .
  • Kay , J. , Lesser , R. and Coltheart , M. 1992 . Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia , Hove, , UK : Psychology Press .
  • Kertesz , A. and Benson , D. F. 1970 . Neologistic jargon . Cortex , 6 : 362 – 386 .
  • Kohn , S. E. and Smith , K. L. 1994 . Distinctions between two phonological output deficits . Applied Psycholinguistics , 15 : 75 – 95 .
  • Levelt , W. J. M. 1989 . Speaking , Cambridge, MA : MIT Press .
  • Levelt , W. J. M. , Roelofs , A. and Meyer , A. S. 1999 . A theory of lexical access in speech production . Behavioural and Brain Sciences , 22 : 1 – 38 .
  • Levelt , W. J. M. and Wheeldon , L. 1994 . Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary . Cognition , 50 : 239 – 269 .
  • Marshall , J. 2006 . Jargon aphasia: What have we learned? . Aphasiology , 20 : 387 – 410 .
  • McCloskey , M. , Macaruso , P. and Rapp , B. 2005 . Grapheme-to-lexeme feedback in the cognitive spelling system . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 23 : 278 – 307 .
  • Miller , D. and Ellis , A. 1987 . “ Speech and writing errors in “neologistic jargonaphasia”: A lexical activation hypothesis ” . In The cognitive neuropsychology of language , Edited by: Coltheart , M. , Sartori , G. and Job , R. Hove, , UK : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd .
  • Moses , M. S. , Nickels , L. A. and Sheard , C. 2004 . Disentangling the web: Neologistic perseverative errors in jargon aphasia . Neurocase , 10 : 452 – 461 .
  • Ohala , J. J. 1990 . There is no interface between phonology and phonetics . Journal of Phonetics , 18 : 153 – 171 .
  • Paivio , A. 1991 . Dual coding theory—retrospect and current status . Canadian Journal of Psychology–Revue Canadienne de Psychologie , 45 : 255 – 287 .
  • Perecman , E. and Brown , J. 1985 . Varieties of aphasic jargon . Language Sciences , 7 : 176 – 215 .
  • Rapp , B. and Caramazza , A. 1993 . On the distinction between deficits of access and deficits of storage—a question of theory . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 10 : 113 – 141 .
  • Rapp , B. and Goldrick , M. 2000 . Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production . Psychological Review , 107 : 460 – 499 .
  • Rapp , B. and Goldrick , M. 2004 . Feedback by any other name is still interactivity: A reply to Roelofs (2004) . Psychological Review , 111 : 573 – 578 .
  • Riddoch , M. J. and Humphreys , G. W. 1993 . Birmingham Object Recognition Battery , Hove, , UK : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd .
  • Robson , J. , Pring , T. , Marshall , J. and Chiat , S. 2003 . Phoneme frequency effects in jargon aphasia: A phonological investigation of nonword errors . Brain and Language , 85 : 109 – 124 .
  • Roelofs , A. On-line versus off-line priming of word-form encoding in spoken word production . Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society . Edited by: Ram , A. and Eiselt , K. pp. 772 – 777 . Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc .
  • Roelofs , A. 1999 . Phonological segments and features as planning units in speech production . Language and Cognitive Processes , 14 : 173 – 200 .
  • Roelofs , A. 2004 . Comprehension-based versus production-internal feedback-in planning spoken words: A rejoinder to Rapp and Goldrick (2004) . Psychological Review , 111 : 579 – 580 .
  • Roelofs , A. , Meyer , A. S. and Levelt , W. J. M. 1998 . A case for the lemma/lexeme distinction in models of speaking: Comment on Caramazza and Miozzo (1997) . Cognition , 69 : 219 – 230 .
  • Romani , C. and Calabrese , A. 1998 . Syllabic constraints in the phonological errors of an aphasic patient . Brain & Language , 64 : 83 – 121 .
  • Romani , C. and Galluzzi , C. 2005 . The effects of syllabic complexity on correct repetition in a group of aphasic patients . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 22 : 817 – 850 .
  • Romani , C. , Olson , A. , Semenza , C. and Grana , A. 2002 . Patterns of phonological errors as a function of a phonological versus an articulatory locus of impairment . Cortex , 38 : 541 – 567 .
  • Schwartz , M. F. , Dell , G. S. , Martin , N. , Gahl , S. and Sobel , P. 2006 . A case-series test of the interactive two-step model of lexical access: Evidence from picture naming . Journal of Memory and Language , 54 : 228 – 264 .
  • Schwartz , M. F. , Wilshire , C. E. , Gagnon , D. A. and Polansky , M. 2004 . Origins of nonword phonological errors in aphasic picture naming . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 21 : 159 – 186 .
  • Shallice , T. , Rumiati , R. I. and Zadini , A. 2000 . The selective impairment of the phonological output buffer . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 17 : 517 – 546 .
  • Shattuck-Hufnagel , S. and Klatt , D. H. 1979 . The limited use of distinctive features and markedness in speech production: Evidence from speech error data . Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , 18 : 41 – 55 .
  • Snodgrass , J. G. and Vanderwart , M. 1980 . A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 6 : 174 – 215 .
  • Stemberger , J. P. 1982 . The nature of segments in the lexicon: Evidence from speech errors . Lingua , 56 : 235 – 259 .
  • Stenneken , P. , Hofmann , M. and Jacobs , A. M. 2005 . Patterns of phoneme and syllable frequency in jargon aphasia . Brain and Language , 95 : 221 – 222 .
  • Ward , J. and Romani , C. 1998 . Serial position effects and lexical activation in spelling: Evidence from a single case study . Neurocase , 4 : 189 – 206 .
  • Wilson , M. D. 1988 . The MRC Psycholinguistic Database: Machine readable dictionary, Version 2 . Behavioural Research Methods, Instruments and Computers , 20 : 6 – 11 .

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.