1,680
Views
17
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Cognitive neuropsychology and its vicissitudes: The fate of Caramazza's axioms

Pages 385-411 | Received 19 Feb 2015, Accepted 09 Dec 2015, Published online: 29 Jun 2016

References

  • Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., & Tranel, D. (1990). Neuropsychological impairments associated with lesions caused by tumor or stroke. Archives of Neurology, 47, 397–405. doi:10.1001/archneur.1990.00530040039017
  • Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). In G. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (pp. 47–89). New York: Academic.
  • Badecker, W., & Caramazza, A. (1985). On considerations of method and theory governing the use of clinical categories in neurolinguistics and cognitive neuropsychology: The case against agrammatism. Cognition, 20, 97–125. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90049-6
  • Beauvois, M. F., & Derouesne, J. (1981). Lexical or orthographic agraphia. Brain, 104, 21–49. doi:10.1093/brain/104.1.21
  • Berndt, R. S., & Caramazza, A. (1980). Semantic operations deficits in sentence comprehension. Psychological Research, 41, 169–177. doi:10.1007/BF00308654
  • Boden, M. (2006). Mind as machine: A history of cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Brambati, S. M., Myers, D., Wilson, A., Rankin, K. P., Allison, S. C., Rosen, H. J. et al. (2006). The anatomy of category-specific object naming in neurodegenerative diseases. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1644–1653. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1644
  • Bruce, V., & Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 305–327. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02199.x
  • Campanella, F., D'Agostini, S., Skrap, M., & Shallice, T. (2010). Naming manipulable objects: Anatomy of a category specific effect in left temporal tumours. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1583–1597. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.002
  • Campanella, F., Mondani, M., Skrap, M., & Shallice, T. (2008). Semantic access dysphasia resulting from left temporal lobe tumours. Brain, 132, 87–102. doi:10.1093/brain/awn302
  • Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Mahon, B., & Caramazza, A. (2003). What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits? A critical review of the clinical Evidence. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 213–261. doi:10.1080/02643290244000266
  • Cappa, S. F. (2000). Recovery from aphasia: Why and how? Brain and Language, 71, 39–41. doi:10.1006/brln.1999.2206
  • Caramazza, A. (1986). On drawing inferences about the structure of normal cognitive systems from the analysis of patterns of impaired performance: The case for single-patient studies. Brain and Cognition, 5, 41–66. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(86)90061-8
  • Caramazza, A., & Coltheart, M. (2006). Cognitive neuropsychology twenty years on. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 3–12. doi: 10.1080/02643290500443250
  • Caramazza, A., & McCloskey, M. (1988). The case for single-patient studies. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 517–527. doi:10.1080/02643298808253271
  • Caramazza, A., & Miceli, G. (1990). The structure of graphemic representations. Cognition, 37, 243–297. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90047-N
  • Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. B. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3, 572–582. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(76)90048-1
  • Caramazza, A., Capitani, E., Rey, A., & Berndt, R. S. (2001). Agrammatic Broca's aphasia is not associated with a single pattern of comprehension performance. Brain and Language, 76, 158–184. doi:10.1006/brln.1999.2275
  • Caramazza, A., Miceli, G., Villa, G., & Romani, C. (1987). The role of the graphemic buffer in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia. Cognition, 26, 59–85. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(87)90014-X
  • Chertkow, H., Bub, D., Deaudon, C., & Whitehead, V. (1997). On the status of object concepts in aphasia. Brain and Language, 58, 203–232. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1771
  • Cipolotti, L., Healy, C., Chan, E., Bolsover, F., Lecce, F. White, M. E., … , Bozzali, M. (2015a). The impact of different aetiologies on the cognitive performance of frontal patients. Neuropsychologia, 68, 21–30. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.12.025
  • Cipolotti, L., Healy, C., Chan, E., Macpherson, S., White, M., Woollett, K., Turner, M. et al. (2015b). The effect of age on cognitive performance of frontal patients. Neuropsychologia, 75, 233–241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.011
  • Coltheart, M. (1980). Deep dyslexia: A review of the syndrome. In M. Coltheart, K. E. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds), Deep dyslexia (pp. 22–47). London: Routledge.
  • Coltheart, M. (2004). Brain imaging, connectionism, and cognitive neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 21–25. doi:10.1080/02643290342000159
  • Coltheart, M. (2006a). Acquired dyslexias and the computational modelling of reading. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 96–109. doi:10.1080/02643290500202649
  • Coltheart, M. (2006b). What has functional neuroimaging told us about the mind (so far)? (Position Paper Presented to the European Cognitive Neuropsychology Workshop, Bressanone, 2005) Cortex, 42, 323–331. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70358-7
  • Coltheart, M. (2013). How can functional neuroimaging inform cognitive theories? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 98–103. doi:10.1177/1745691612469208
  • Coltheart, M., Patterson, K., & Marshall, J. (1980). Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches.. Psychological Review, 100, 589–608. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.589
  • Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Tranel, D., Hichwa, R. D., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature, 380, 499–505. doi:10.1038/380499a0
  • Dell, G., Schwartz, M., Martin, N., Saffran, E., & Gagnon, D. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104, 801–838. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.801
  • Farah, M., & McClelland, J. (1991). A computational model of semantic memory impairment: Modality-specificity and emergent category-specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 339–357. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.120.4.339
  • Farah, M. J., O'Reilly, R. C., & Vecera, S. P. (1993). Dissociated overt and covert recognition as an emergent property of a lesioned neural network. Psychological Review, 100, 571–588. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.571
  • Fischer-Baum, S. (2013). Making sense of deviance: Identifying dissociating cases within the case series approach. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 30, 597–617. doi:10.1080/02643294.2013.846903
  • Fischer-Baum, S., & Rapp, B. (2012). Underlying cause(s) of letter perseveration errors. Neuropsychologia, 50, 305–318. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.001
  • Forde, E. M., & Humphreys, G. W. (2007). Contrasting effects of repetition across tasks: Implications for understanding the nature of refractory behavior and models of semantic memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 198–211. doi:10.3758/CABN.7.3.198
  • Forkel, S. J., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Dell'Acqua, F., Kalra, L., Murphy, D. G. M., Williams, S. C. R., & Catani, M. (2014). Anatomical predictors of aphasia recovery: A tractography study of bilateral perisylvian language networks. Brain, 137 (7), 2027–2039. doi:10.1093/brain/awu113
  • Gardner, H. E., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Dodds, N., Jones, T., Ehsan, S., & Jefferies, E. (2012). The differential contributions of PFC and temporo-parietal cortex to multimodal semantic control: Exploring refractory effects in semantic aphasia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 778–793. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00184
  • Gilaie-Dotan, S., Saygin, A. P., Lorenzi, L. J., Rees, G. and Behrmann, M. (in press). Ventral aspect of the visual form pathway is not critical for the perception of biological motion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. doi:10.1073/pnas.1414974112
  • Gotts, S. J., & Plaut, D. C. (2002). The impact of synaptic depression following brain damage: A connectionist account of "access/refractory" and "degraded-store" semantic impairments. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 187–213. doi: 10.3758/CABN.2.3.187
  • Harley, T. (2004). Promises, promises. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 51–56. doi: 10.1080/02643290342000212
  • Henson, R. (2005). What can functional neuroimaging tell the experimental psychologist? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58, 193–233. doi:10.1080/02724980443000502
  • Hinton, G., & Shallice, T. (1991). Lesioning an attractor network: Investigations of acquired dyslexia. Psychological Review, 98, 74–95. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.1.74
  • Hoffman, P., Evans, G. A. L., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2014). The anterior temporal lobes are critically involved in acquiring new conceptual knowledge: Evidence for impaired feature integration in semantic dementia. Cortex, 50, 19–31. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2013.10.006
  • Houghton, G., Glasspool, D. W., & Shallice, T. (1994). Spelling and serial recall: Insights from a competitive queuing model. In G. Brown & N. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 367–404). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
  • Jefferies, E., Baker, S. S., Doran, M., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2007). Refractory effects in stroke aphasia: A consequence of poor semantic control. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1065–1079. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.009
  • Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Semantic impairment in stroke aphasia versus semantic dementia: A case-series comparison. Brain, 129, 2132–2147. doi:10.1093/brain/awl153
  • Jefferies, E., Rogers, T. T., & Ralph, M. A. (2011). Premorbid expertise produces category-specific impairment in a domain-general semantic disorder. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3213–3223. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.024
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, Ma: Harvard University Press.
  • Karnath, H. O., & Steinbach, J. P. (2011). Do brain tumours allow valid conclusions on the localisation of human brain functions? – Objections. Cortex, 47, 1004–1006. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.08.006
  • Khemlani, S., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2012). Theories of the syllogism: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 427–457. doi:10.1037/a0026841
  • Kolk, H., & Heeschen, C. (1992). Agrammatism, paragrammatism and the management of language. Language and Cognitive Processes, 7, 89–129. doi:10.1080/01690969208409381
  • Lambon Ralph, M. A., Lowe, C., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Neural basis of category-specific semantic deficits for living things: Evidence from semantic dementia, HSVE and a neural network model. Brain, 130, 1127–1137. doi:10.1093/brain/awm025
  • Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Plaut, D. C. (2011). Finite case series or infinite single-case studies? Comments on "Case series investigations in cognitive neuropsychology" by Schwartz and Dell (2010). Cognitive Neuropsychology, 28, 466–474. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2012.671765
  • Laws, K. R. (2005). "Illusions of normality": A methodological critique of category-specific naming. Cortex, 41, 842–851. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70303-4
  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Freeman.
  • Marshall, J., & Newcombe, F. (1966). Syntactic and semantic errors in paralexia. Neuropsychologia 4, 169–176. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(66)90045-5
  • Marshall, J., & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 175–199. doi:10.1007/BF01067101
  • McCloskey, M., & Caramazza, A. (1988). Theory and methodology in cognitive neuropsychology: A response to our critics. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 583–623. doi:10.1080/02643298808253276
  • McCloskey, M., & Schubert, T. (2014). Shared versus separate processes for letter and digit identification. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31, 437–460. doi:10.1080/02643294.2013.869202
  • Milner, B. (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In J.M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • Mion, M., Patterson, K., Acosta-Cabronero, J., Pengas, G., Izquierdo-Garcia, D. Hong, Y. T., … , Nestor, P. J. (2010). What the left and right anterior fusiform gyri tell us about semantic memory. Brain, 133, 3256–3268. doi:10.1093/brain/awq272
  • Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Mozer, M., & Behrmann, M. (1990). On the interaction of selective attention and lexical knowledge: A connectionist account of neglect dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 96–123. doi:10.1162/jocn.1990.2.2.96
  • Mozer, M., & Behrmann, M. (1992). Reading with attentional impairments: A brain-damaged model of neglect and attentional dyslexia. In R. G. Reilly & N.E. Sharkey (Eds.), Connectionist approaches to natural language processing (pp. 409–460). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Murdock, B. (1974). Human memory: Theory and data. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Patterson, K. (2007). The reign of typicality in semantic memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 813–821. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2090
  • Patterson, K., Coltheart, M., & Marshall, J. (1985). Surface dyslexia. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Patterson, K., & Plaut, D. C. (2009). “Shallow draughts intoxicate the brain”: Lessons from cognitive science for cognitive neuropsychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 39–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01012.x
  • Patterson, K. E., Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). Connections and disconnections: Acquired dyslexia in a computational model of reading processes. In R. G. M. Morris (Ed.), Parallel distributed processing: Implications for psychology and neuroscience. London: Oxford University Press.
  • Plaut, D. C. (1999). A connectionist approach to word reading and acquired dyslexia: Extension to sequential processing. Cognitive Science, 23, 543–568. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2304_7
  • Plaut, D. C. (2002). Graded modality-specific specialisation in semantics: A computational account of optic aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 603–639. doi:10.1080/026432902440
  • Plaut, D. C., & Shallice, T. (1993a). Deep dyslexia: A case study of connectionist neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 377–500. doi:10.1080/02643299308253469
  • Plaut, D. C., & Shallice, T. (1993b). Perseverative and semantic influences on visual object naming errors in optic aphasia: A connectionist account. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 89–117. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.89
  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J., Seidenberg, M., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–115. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.56
  • Posner, M. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Englewood Heights, NJ: Erlbaum .
  • Rapp, B., & Goldrick, M. (2000). Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychological Review, 107, 460–499. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.460
  • Robinson, G., Blair, J., & Cipolotti, L. (1998). Dynamic aphasia: An inability to select between competing verbal responses? Brain, 121(1), 77–89. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.1.77
  • Robinson, G., Shallice, T., Bozzali, M., & Cipolotti, L. (2012). The differing roles of the frontal cortex in fluency tests. Brain, 135, 2202–2214. doi:10.1093/brain/aws142
  • Roca, M., Parr, A., Thompson, R., Woolgar, A., Torralva, T., Antoun, N., & Duncan, J. (2010). Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Brain, 133, 234–247. doi:10.1093/brain/awp269
  • Rogers, T. T., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Garrard, P., Bozeat, S., McClelland, J. L., Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (2004). Structure and deterioration of semantic memory: A neuropsychological and computational investigation. Psychological Review, 111, 205–235. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.205
  • Rosenbaum, R. S., Gilboa, A., & Moscovitch, M. (2014). Case studies continue to illuminate the cognitive neuroscience of memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316, 105–133. doi:10.1111/nyas.12467
  • Ruml, W., & Caramazza, A. (2000). An evaluation of a computational model of lexical access: Comment on Dell et al. (1997). Psychological Review, 107, 609–634. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.609
  • Sartori, G., Gnoato, F., Mariani, I., Prioni, S., & Lombardi, L. (2007). Semantic relevance, domain specificity and the sensory/functional theory of category-specificity. Neuropsychologia, 45, 966–976. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.08.028
  • Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (1994). What are the memory systems of 1994? In D.L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems (pp. 3–38). Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
  • Schnider, A. (2003). Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of thought to ongoing reality. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 662–671. doi:10.1038/nrn1179
  • Schnider, A. (2008). The confabulating mind: How the brain creates reality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Schnider, A., von Daniken, C., & Gutbrod, K. (1996). The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations. Brain, 119(4), 1365–1375. doi:10.1093/brain/119.4.1365
  • Schnur, T. T., Schwartz, M. F., Brecher, A., & Hodgson, C. (2006). Semantic interference during blocked-cyclic naming: Evidence from aphasia?. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 199–227. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.10.002
  • Schwartz, M. F., & Dell, G. S. (2010). Case series investigations in cognitive neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27, 477–494. doi:10.1080/02643294.2011.574111
  • Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E., & Marin, O. (1980). The word order problem in agrammatism. Brain and Language, 10, 249–262. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(80)90055-3
  • Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 20, 11–21. doi:10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11
  • Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Shallice, T. (2014). On compensatory strategies and computational models: The case of pure alexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31, 529–543. doi:10.1080/02643294.2014.914023
  • Shallice, T., & Buiatti, T. (2011). Types of case series--the anatomically based approach: Commentary on M. F. Schwartz & G. S. Dell: case series investigations in cognitive neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 28, 500–514. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2012.674012
  • Shallice, T., & Cooper, R. P. (2011). The organisation of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Shallice, T., & Gillingham, S. (2012). On neuropsychological studies of prefrontal cortex: The Robbia approach. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (2nd ed., pp. 475–489). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Shallice, T., Mussoni, A., D'Agostino, S., & Skrap, M. (2010). Right posterior cortical functions in a tumour patient series. Cortex, 46, 1178–1188. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.04.005
  • Shallice, T., & Skrap, M. (2011). Localisation through operation for brain tumour: A reply to Karnath and Steinbach. Cortex, 47, 1007–1009. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.12.006
  • Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. (1970). Independent functioning of the verbal memory stores: A neuropsychological study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 261–273. doi:10.1080/00335557043000203
  • Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. K. (1977). Auditory-verbal short-term memory impairment and conduction aphasia. Brain and Language, 4, 479–491. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(77)90040-2
  • Staller, J., Buchanan, D., Singer, M., Lappin, J., & Webb, W. (1978). Alexia without agraphia: An experimental case study. Brain and Language, 5, 378–387. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(78)90033-0
  • Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P. (2007). Is there a dysexecutive syndrome? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 901–915. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2096
  • Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Hamer, L., Palumbo, C., Dempster, R., Binns, M. et al. (1998). The effects of focal anterior and posterior brain lesions on verbal fluency. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 265–278.
  • Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Shallice, T., Picton, T. W., Binns, M. A., Macdonald, R., … , Katz, D.I. (2005). Multiple frontal systems controlling response speed. Neuropsychologia, 43, 396–417. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.010
  • Tressoldi, P. E., Sella, F., Coltheart, M., & Umilta, C. (2012). Using functional neuroimaging to test theories of cognition: A selective survey of studies from 2007 to 2011 as a contribution to the Decade of the Mind Initiative. Cortex, 48, 1247–1250. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.024
  • Tyler, L. K., Stamatakis, E. A., Post, B., Randall, B., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2005). Temporal and frontal systems in speech comprehension: An fMRI study of past tense processing. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1963–1974. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.008
  • Vallesi, A. (2012). Organisation of executive functions: Hemispheric asymmetries. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 367–386. doi:10.1080/20445911.2012.678992
  • Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635–657. doi:10.1080/14640747508400525
  • Warrington, E. K., & Cipolotti, L. (1996). Word comprehension. The distinction between refractory and storage impairments. Brain, 119(2), 611–625. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.611
  • Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. (1983). Category specific access dysphasia. Brain, 106(4), 859–878. doi: 10.1093/brain/106.4.859
  • Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. A. (1987). Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110(5), 1273–1296. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.5.1273
  • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1969). The selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. Brain, 92, 885–896. doi:10.1093/brain/92.4.885
  • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1980). Word-form dyslexia. Brain, 103, 99–112. doi:10.1093/brain/103.1.99
  • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107(3), 829–853. doi: 10.1093/brain/107.3.829
  • Warrington, E. K., & Taylor, A. M. (1973). The contribution of the right parietal lobe to object recognition. Cortex, 9, 152–164. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(73)80024-3
  • Welbourne, S. R., Woollams, A. M., Crisp, J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). The role of plasticity-related functional reorganization in the explanation of central dyslexias. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 28, 65–108. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2011.621937
  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1968). Sparing of short-term memory in an amnesic patient: Implications for strength theory of memory. Neuropsychologia, 6, 235–244. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(68)90022-5
  • Wixted, J. T., & Mickes, L. (2013). On the relationship between fMRI and theories of cognition: The arrow points in both directions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 104–107. doi: 10.1177/1745691612469022
  • Woollams, A. M., Hoffman, P., Roberts, D. J., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Patterson, K. E. (2014). What lies beneath: A comparison of reading aloud in pure alexia and semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31, 461–481. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2014.882300
  • Woollams, A. M., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Plaut, D. C., & Patterson, K. (2007). SD-squared: On the association between semantic dementia and surface dyslexia. Psychological Review, 114, 316–339. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.316

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.