200
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Translational Cognitive Neuropsychology

Challenges to developing time-based signal detection models for word production

&
Pages 85-88 | Received 04 Jun 2018, Accepted 03 Feb 2019, Published online: 27 Feb 2019

References

  • Anders, R., Alario, F.-X., & Van Maanen, L. (2016). The shifted Wald distribution for response time data analysis. Psychological Methods, 21, 309–327. doi: 10.1037/met0000066
  • Anders, R., Riès, S., van Maanen, L., & Alario, F.-X. (2015). Evidence accumulation as a model for lexical selection. Cognitive Psychology, 82, 57–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.07.002
  • Anders, R., Riès, S., van Maanen, L., & Alario, F.-X. (2017). Lesions to the left lateral prefrontal cortex impair decision threshold adjustment for lexical selection. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34, 1–20. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2017.1282447
  • Chen, Q., & Mirman, D. (2012). Competition and cooperation among similar representations: Toward a unified account of facilitative and inhibitory effects of lexical neighbors. Psychological Review, 119, 417–430. doi: 10.1037/a0027175
  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
  • Dell, G. S., & Gordon, J. K. (2003). Neighbors in the lexicon: Friend or foe? In N. O. Schiller & A. S. Meyer (Eds.), Phonetics and phonology in language comprehension and production: Differences and similarities (pp. 9–37). New York: Walter de Gruyter.
  • Donkin, C., Brown, S., Heathcote, A., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2011). Diffusion versus linear ballistic accumulation: Different models but the same conclusions about psychological processes? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 61–69. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0022-4
  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–38.
  • Nozari, N., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (2011). Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production. Cognitive Psychology, 63, 1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.05.001
  • Nozari, N., & Hepner, C. R. (2018). To select or to wait? The importance of criterion setting in debates of competitive lexical selection. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3–40. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2018.1476335
  • Oppenheim, G. M., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (2010). The dark side of incremental learning: A model of cumulative semantic interference during lexical access in speech production. Cognition, 114, 227–252. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.007
  • Pew, R. W. (1969). The speed-accuracy operating characteristic. Acta Psychologica, 30, 16–26. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(69)90035-3
  • Pike, R. (1973). Response latency models for signal detection. Psychological Review, 80, 53–68. doi: 10.1037/h0033871
  • Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.85.2.59
  • Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (2008). The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Computation, 20, 873–922. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420
  • Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Diffusion decision model: Current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 260–281. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.007
  • Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42, 107–142. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90041-F
  • Usher, M., Olami, Z., & McClelland, J. L. (2002). Hick’s law in a stochastic race model with speed–accuracy tradeoff. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46, 704–715. doi: 10.1006/jmps.2002.1420
  • Van Maanen, L., & Van Rijn, H. (2007). An accumulator model of semantic interference. Cognitive Systems Research, 8, 174–181. doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2007.05.002
  • Voss, A., Rothermund, K., & Voss, J. (2004). Interpreting the parameters of the diffusion model: An empirical validation. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1206–1220. doi: 10.3758/BF03196893

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.