2,121
Views
35
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Grounding language in the neglected senses of touch, taste, and smell

&
Pages 363-392 | Received 30 Oct 2018, Accepted 20 May 2019, Published online: 23 Jun 2019

References

  • Ackerman, D. (1990). A natural history of the senses. London, UK: Vintage Books.
  • Ackerman, J. M., Nocera, C. C., & Bargh, J. A. (2010). Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and decisions. Science, 328(5986), 1712–1715. doi: 10.1126/science.1189993
  • Aguirre, G. K. (2003). Functional imaging in behavioral neurology and cognitive neuropsychology. In behavioral neurology and cognitive neuropsychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(04), 245–266. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10000853
  • Andrade, J., May, J., Deeprose, C., Baugh, S.-J., & Ganis, G. (2014). Assessing vividness of mental imagery: The Plymouth sensory imagery Questionnaire. British Journal of Psychology, 105(4), 547–563. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12050
  • Arshamian, A., Iannilli, E., Gerber, J. C., Willander, J., Persson, J., Seo, H.-S., … Larsson, M. (2013). The functional neuroanatomy of odor evoked autobiographical memories cued by odors and words. Neuropsychologia, 51(1), 123–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.023
  • Arshamian, A., & Larsson, M. (2014). Same same but different: The case of olfactory imagery. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(34), 1–8.
  • Ashton, M. C., Pilkington, A. C., & Lee, K. (2014). Do prosocial people prefer sweet-tasting foods? An attempted replication of Meier, Moeller, Riemer-Peltz, and Robinson (2012). Journal of Research in Personality, 52, 42–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.06.006
  • Auvray, M., Myin, E., & Spence, C. (2010). The sensory-discriminative and affective-motivational aspects of pain. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(2), 214–223. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.008
  • Auvray, M., & Spence, C. (2008). The multisensory perception of flavor. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 1016–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.06.005
  • Backhouse, A. E. (1994). The lexical field of taste: A semantic study of Japanese taste terms. Cambridge; New York, N.Y., USA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bak, T. H., O’Donovan, D. G., Xuereb, J. H., Boniface, S., & Hodges, J. R. (2001). Selective impairment of verb processing associated with pathological changes in Brodmann areas 44 and 45 in the motor neurone disease–dementia–aphasia syndrome. Brain, 124(1), 103–120. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.1.103
  • Bargh, J. A., & Shalev, I. (2012). The substitutability of physical and social warmth in daily life. Emotion, 12(1), 154–162. doi: 10.1037/a0023527
  • Barros-Loscertales, A., Gonzalez, J., Pulvermuller, F., Ventura-Campos, N., Bustamante, J. C., Costumero, V., … Avila, C. (2012). Reading Salt activates gustatory brain regions: fMRI evidence for semantic grounding in a novel sensory modality. Cerebral Cortex, 22(11), 2554–2563. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr324
  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(04), 577–660.
  • Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1435), 1177–1187. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2003.1319
  • Barsalou, L. W. (2016). On staying grounded and avoiding quixotic dead ends. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1122–1142. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1028-3
  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295
  • Bedny, M., & Caramazza, A. (2011). Perception, action, and word meanings in the human brain: The case from action verbs: Word meanings and sensory experience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224(1), 81–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06013.x
  • Beilock, S. L., Lyons, I. M., Mattarella-Micke, A., Nusbaum, H. C., & Small, S. L. (2008). Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(36), 13269–13273. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803424105
  • Bensafi, M., Porter, J., Pouliot, S., Mainland, J., Johnson, B., Zelano, C., … Sobel, N. (2003). Olfactomotor activity during imagery mimics that during perception. Nature Neuroscience, 6(11), 1142–1144. doi: 10.1038/nn1145
  • Bensafi, M., Pouliot, S., & Sobel, N. (2005). Odorant-specific patterns of sniffing during imagery distinguish ‘bad’ and ‘good’ olfactory imagers. Chemical Senses, 30(6), 521–529. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bji045
  • Bensafi, M., Sobel, N., & Khan, R. M. (2007). Hedonic-specific activity in piriform cortex during odor imagery mimics that during odor perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98(6), 3254–3262. doi: 10.1152/jn.00349.2007
  • Bergen, B., Lindsay, S., Matlock, T., & Narayanan, S. (2007). Spatial and linguistic aspects of visual imagery in sentence comprehension. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 31(5), 733–764. doi: 10.1080/03640210701530748
  • Bergman, P., Ho, H.-N., Koizumi, A., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., & Kitagawa, N. (2015). The pleasant heat? Evidence for thermal-emotional implicit associations occurring with semantic and physical thermal stimulation. Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(1), 24–30. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2014.988132
  • Boddy, P., Paz-Alonso, K., & Yee, E. (2016). Does smelling pine cones make it harder to think about strawberries? Presented at the Annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Boston.
  • Boesveldt, S., & de Graaf, K. (2017). The differential role of smell and taste for eating behavior. Perception, 46(3–4), 307–319. doi: 10.1177/0301006616685576
  • Bonch-Osmolovskaya, A., Rakhilina, E., & Reznikova, T. (2009). Conceptualization of pain: A database for lexical typology. Logic, Language, and Computation, 5422, 110–123. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-00665-4_10
  • Borelli, E., Crepaldi, D., Porro, C. A., & Cacciari, C. (2018). The psycholinguistic and affective structure of words conveying pain. PLOS ONE, 13(6), e0199658. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199658
  • Borhani, K., Làdavas, E., Fotopoulou, A., & Haggard, P. (2017). “Lacking warmth”: Alexithymia trait is related to warm-specific thermal somatosensory processing. Biological Psychology, 128, 132–140. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.07.012
  • Brunyé, T. T., Walters, E. K., Ditman, T., Gagnon, S. A., Mahoney, C. R., & Taylor, H. A. (2012). The fabric of thought: Priming tactile properties during reading influences direct tactile perception. Cognitive Science, 36(8), 1449–1467. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01268.x
  • Buck, C. D. (1949). A dictionary of selected synonyms in the principal Indo-European languages: A contribution to the history of ideas. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Burenhult, N., & Majid, A. (2011). Olfaction in Aslian ideology and language. The Senses & Society, 6(1), 19–29. doi: 10.2752/174589311X12893982233597
  • Bushdid, C., Magnasco, M. O., Vosshall, L. B., & Keller, A. (2014). Humans can discriminate more than 1 trillion olfactory stimuli. Science, 343(6177), 1370–1372. doi: 10.1126/science.1249168
  • Cain, W. S. (1979). To know with the nose: Keys to odor identification. Science, 203(4379), 467–470. doi: 10.1126/science.760202
  • Caramazza, A., Anzellotti, S., Strnad, L., & Lingnau, A. (2014). Embodied cognition and mirror neurons: A critical assessment. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37(1), 1–15. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-013950
  • Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 351–367. doi: 10.1037/a0015854
  • Chabris, C. F., Heck, P. R., Mandart, J., Benjamin, D. J., & Simons, D. J. (2019). No evidence that experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Social Psychology, 50(2), 127–132. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000361
  • Chamberlain, A. F. (1903). Primitive taste words. American Journal of Psychology, 14(3/4), 146–153.
  • Chan, K. Q., Tong, E. M. W., Tan, D. H., & Koh, A. H. Q. (2013). What do love and jealousy taste like? Emotion, 13(6), 1142–1149. doi: 10.1037/a0033758
  • Chao, L. L., & Martin, A. (1999). Cortical regions associated with perceiving, naming, and knowing about colors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(1), 25–35. doi: 10.1162/089892999563229
  • Chatterjee, A. (2010). Disembodying cognition. Language and Cognition, 2(1), 79–116.
  • Chen, I.-H., Zhao, Q., Long, Y., Lu, Q., & Huang, C.-R. (2019). Mandarin Chinese modality exclusivity norms. PLOS ONE, 14(2), e0211336. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0211336.
  • Chow, H. M., Mar, R. A., Xu, Y., Liu, S., Wagage, S., & Braun, A. R. (2015). Personal experience with narrated events modulates functional connectivity within visual and motor systems during story comprehension: Effect of experience on functional connectivity. Human Brain Mapping, 36(4), 1494–1505. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22718
  • Christensen, C. M. (1983). Effects of color on aroma, flavor and texture judgments of foods. Journal of Food Science, 48(3), 787–790. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2621.1983.tb14899.x
  • Citron, F. M. M., & Goldberg, A. E. (2014). Metaphorical sentences are more emotionally engaging than their literal counterparts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(11), 2585–2595. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00654
  • Classen, C. (1997). Foundations for an anthropology of the senses. International Social Science Journal, 49(153), 401–412. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2451.1997.tb00032.x
  • Classen, C., Howes, D., & Synnott, A. (1994). Aroma: The cultural history of smell. London: Routledge.
  • Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2010). Look but don’t touch: Tactile disadvantage in processing modality-specific words. Cognition, 115(1), 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.005
  • Connell, L., Lynott, D., & Banks, B. (2018). Interoception: The forgotten modality in perceptual grounding of abstract and concrete concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170143. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0143
  • Connell, L., Lynott, D., & Dreyer, F. (2012). A functional role for modality-specific perceptual systems in conceptual representations. PLoS ONE, 7(3), e33321. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033321
  • Craig, A. (2003). A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion. Trends in Neurosciences, 26(6), 303–307. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00123-1
  • Croijmans, I., & Majid, A. (2015). Odor naming is difficult, even for wine and coffee experts. Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Presented at the CogSci 2015, Pasadena, CA.
  • Croijmans, I. M., Speed, L. J., Arshamian, A., & Majid, A. (2019). Measuring the multisensory imagery of wine: The Vividness of Wine Imagery Questionnaire (VWIQ). Multisensory Research.
  • Cytowic, R. E. (1995). Synesthesia: Phenomenology and neuropsychology. Psyche, 2(10), 2–10.
  • de Araujo, I. E., Rolls, E. T., Velazco, M. I., Margot, C., & Cayeux, I. (2005). Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron, 46(4), 671–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.021
  • de la Vega, I., de Filippis, M., Lachmair, M., Dudschig, C., & Kaup, B. (2012). Emotional valence and physical space: Limits of interaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(2), 375–385. doi: 10.1037/a0024979
  • Delon-Martin, C., Plailly, J., Fonlupt, P., Veyrac, A., & Royet, J.-P. (2013). Perfumers’ expertise induces structural reorganization in olfactory brain regions. NeuroImage, 68, 55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.044
  • Derbyshire, S. W. G., Whalley, M. G., Stenger, V. A., & Oakley, D. A. (2004). Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain. NeuroImage, 23(1), 392–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.033
  • Desai, R. H., Herter, T., Riccardi, N., Rorden, C., & Fridriksson, J. (2015). Concepts within reach: Action performance predicts action language processing in stroke. Neuropsychologia, 71, 217–224. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.04.006
  • Desai, R. H., Reilly, M., & van Dam, W. (2018). The multifaceted abstract brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0122
  • de Valk, J. M., Wnuk, E., Huisman, J. L. A., & Majid, A. (2017). Odor–color associations differ with verbal descriptors for odors: A comparison of three linguistically diverse groups. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(4), 1171–1179. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1179-2
  • de Vignemont, F. (2017). Pain and touch. The Monist, 100(4), 465–477. doi: 10.1093/monist/onx022
  • de Vignemont, F., & Massin, O. (2015). Touch. In M. Matthen (Ed.), Touch (pp. 294–314). doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199600472.013.017.
  • Dillmann, J., Miltner, W. H. R., & Weiss, T. (2000). The influence of semantic priming on event-related potentials to painful laser-heat stimuli in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 4, 53–56. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00957-5
  • Dingemanse, M., & Majid, A. (2012). The semantic structure of sensory vocabulary in an African language. 300–305.
  • Dove, G. (2009). Beyond perceptual symbols: A call for representational pluralism. Cognition, 110(3), 412–431. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.016
  • Eck, J., Richter, M., Straube, T., Miltner, W. H. R., & Weiss, T. (2011). Affective brain regions are activated during the processing of pain-related words in migraine patients. Pain, 152(5), 1104–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.01.026
  • Engen, T. (1987). Remembering odors and their names. American Scientist, 75(5), 497–503.
  • Estes, Z., Verges, M., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Head up, foot down: Object words orient attention to the objects’ typical location. Psychological Science, 19(2), 93–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02051.x
  • Fernandino, L., Conant, L. L., Binder, J. R., Blindauer, K., Hiner, B., Spangler, K., & Desai, R. H. (2013). Where is the action? Action sentence processing in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 51(8), 1510–1517. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.008
  • Ferreira, F., Engelhardt, P. E., & Jones, M. W. (2009). Good enough language processing: A satisficing approach. Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Fetterman, A. K., Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2018). On feeling warm and being warm: Daily perceptions of physical warmth fluctuate with interpersonal warmth. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(5), 560–567. doi: 10.1177/1948550617712032
  • Gescheider, G. A. (1997). Psychophysics: The fundamentals. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Gilead, M., Gal, O., Polak, M., & Cholow, Y. (2015). The role of nature and nurture in conceptual metaphors: The case of gustatory priming. Social Psychology, 46(3), 167–173. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000238
  • Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 558–565. doi: 10.3758/BF03196313
  • Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Schneider, W. (2006a). Distinct and common cortical activations for multimodal semantic categories. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(3), 214–222. doi: 10.3758/CABN.6.3.214
  • Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Schneider, W. (2006b). Perceptual knowledge Retrieval activates sensory brain regions. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(18), 4917–4921. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5389-05.2006
  • González, J., Barros-Loscertales, A., Pulvermüller, F., Meseguer, V., Sanjuán, A., Belloch, V., & Ávila, C. (2006). Reading cinnamon activates olfactory brain regions. NeuroImage, 32(2), 906–912. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.037
  • Grabenhorst, F., Rolls, E. T., & Bilderbeck, A. (2008). How cognition modulates affective responses to taste and flavor: Top-down influences on the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortices. Cerebral Cortex, 18(7), 1549–1559. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm185
  • Gu, X., & Han, S. (2007). Neural substrates underlying evaluation of pain in actions depicted in words. Behavioural Brain Research, 181(2), 218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.04.008
  • Han, P., Croy, I., Raue, C., Bensafi, M., Larsson, M., Cavazzana, A., & Hummel, T. (2019). Neural processing of odor-associated words: An fMRI study in patients with acquired olfactory loss. Brain Imaging and Behavior. doi:10.1007/s11682-019-00062-2.
  • Hanks, A. S., Just, D. R., & Brumberg, A. (2016). Marketing vegetables in elementary school cafeterias to increase uptake. Pediatrics, 138(2), e20151720. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-1720
  • Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41(2), 301–307. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9
  • Hauk, O., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the fronto-central cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 21(3), 191–201. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10157
  • Herz, R. S. (2003). The effect of verbal context on olfactory perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(4), 595–606. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.595
  • Herz, R. S., & Clef, J. v. (2001). The influence of verbal labeling on the perception of odors: Evidence for olfactory illusions? Perception, 30(3), 381–391. doi: 10.1068/p3179
  • Ho, H.-N., Van Doorn, G. H., Kawabe, T., Watanabe, J., & Spence, C. (2014). Colour-temperature correspondences: When reactions to thermal stimuli are influenced by colour. PloS One, 9(3), e91854. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091854
  • Hoenig, K., Müller, C., Herrnberger, B., Sim, E.-J., Spitzer, M., Ehret, G., & Kiefer, M. (2011). Neuroplasticity of semantic representations for musical instruments in professional musicians. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1714–1725. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.065
  • Hombert, J.-M. (1992). Terminologie des odeurs dans quelques langues du Gabon. Pholia, 7, 61–63.
  • Howes, D. (2003). Sensual relations: Engaging the senses in culture and social theory. Ann Arbor, Michigan: University of Michigan Press.
  • Iatropoulos, G., Herman, P., Lansner, A., Karlgren, J., Larsson, M., & Olofsson, J. K. (2018). The language of smell: Connecting linguistic and psychophysical properties of odor descriptors. Cognition, 178, 37–49. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.007
  • IJzerman, H., & Semin, G. R. (2009). The thermometer of social relations: Mapping social proximity on temperature. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1214–1220. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02434.x
  • Iwasaki, N., Vinson, D., & Vigliocco, G. (2007). How does it hurt,” kiri-kiri” or” siku-siku"? Japanese mimetic words of pain perceived by Japanese speakers and English speakers. In M. Minami (Ed.), Applying theory and research to learning Japanese as a foreign language (pp. 2–19). Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
  • Kammers, M. P. M., Rose, K., & Haggard, P. (2011). Feeling numb: Temperature, but not thermal pain, modulates feeling of body ownership. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1316–1321. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.039
  • Kaschak, M. P., Madden, C. J., Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., Aveyard, M., Blanchard, A. A., & Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition, 94(3), B79–B89. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.06.005
  • Kaschak, M. P., Zwaan, R. A., Aveyard, M., & Yaxley, R. H. (2006). Perception of auditory motion affects language processing. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 733–744. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_54
  • Kemmerer, D. (2015). Are the motor features of verb meanings represented in the precentral motor cortices? Yes, but within the context of a flexible, multilevel architecture for conceptual knowledge. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(4), 1068–1075. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0784-1
  • Kemmerer, D. (2019). The perspective from semantic typology. In Concepts in the brain: The view from cross-linguistic diversity (pp. 3–28). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Khan, R. M., Luk, C.-H., Flinker, A., Aggarwal, A., Lapid, H., Haddad, R., & Sobel, N. (2007). Predicting odor pleasantness from odorant structure: Pleasantness as a reflection of the physical world. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(37), 10015–10023. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1158-07.2007
  • Kiefer, M., Sim, E.-J., Herrnberger, B., Grothe, J., & Hoenig, K. (2008). The sound of concepts: Four markers for a link between auditory and conceptual brain systems. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(47), 12224–12230. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3579-08.2008
  • Kikuchi, S., Kubota, F., Nisijima, K., Washiya, S., & Kato, S. (2005). Cerebral activation focusing on strong tasting food: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroReport, 16(3), 281–283. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200502280-00016
  • Kita, S. (1997). Two-dimensional semantic analysis of Japanese mimetics. Linguistics, 35(2), 379–416. doi: 10.1515/ling.1997.35.2.379
  • Knost, B., Flor, H., Braun, C., & Birbaumer, N. (1997). Cerebral processing of words and the development of chronic pain. Psychophysiology, 34, 474–481. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02392.x
  • Kobayashi, M., Sasabe, T., Shigihara, Y., Tanaka, M., & Watanabe, Y. (2011). Gustatory imagery reveals functional connectivity from the prefrontal to insular cortices traced with magnetoencephalography. PLoS ONE, 6(7), e21736. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021736
  • Koptjevskaja-Tamm, M. (Ed.). (2015). The linguistics of temperature (Vol. 107). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Kousta, S.-T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 14–34. doi: 10.1037/a0021446
  • Kurby, C. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2013). The activation of modality-specific representations during discourse processing. Brain and Language, 126(3), 338–349. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.07.003
  • Lacey, S., Stilla, R., & Sathian, K. (2012). Metaphorically feeling: Comprehending textural metaphors activates somatosensory cortex. Brain and Language, 120(3), 416–421. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.016
  • Lebois, L. A. M., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., & Barsalou, L. W. (2015). Are automatic conceptual cores the gold standard of semantic processing? The context-dependence of spatial meaning in grounded congruency effects. Cognitive Science, 39(8), 1764–1801. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12174
  • Lee, S. W. S. (2016). Multimodal priming of abstract constructs. Current Opinion in Psychology, 12, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.04.016
  • Lee, D. S., Kim, E., & Schwarz, N. (2015). Something smells fishy: Olfactory suspicion cues improve performance on the Moses illusion and Wason rule discovery task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 59, 47–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.03.006
  • Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2012). Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 737–749. doi: 10.1037/a0029708
  • Levinson, S. C. (2012). The original sin of cognitive science. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(3), 396–403. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01195.x
  • Levinson, S. C., & Majid, A. (2014). Differential ineffability and the senses. Mind & Language, 29(4), 407–427. doi: 10.1111/mila.12057
  • Lorig, T. S. (1999). On the similarity of odor and language perception. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 23(3), 391–398. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(98)00041-4
  • Louwerse, M. M. (2011). Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(2), 273–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01106.x
  • Louwerse, M., & Connell, L. (2011). A taste of words: Linguistic context and perceptual simulation predict the modality of words. Cognitive Science, 35(2), 381–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01157.x
  • Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114(1), 96–104.
  • Ludwig, V. U., & Simner, J. (2013). What colour does that feel? Tactile–visual mapping and the development of cross-modality. Cortex, 49(4), 1089–1099. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.004
  • Luzzi, S., Snowden, J. S., Neary, D., Coccia, M., Provinciali, L., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2007). Distinct patterns of olfactory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease, semantic dementia, frontotemporal dementia, and corticobasal degeneration. Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1823–1831. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.008
  • Lynott, D., & Connell, L. (2009). Modality exclusivity norms for 423 object properties. Behavior Research Methods, 41(2), 558–564. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.2.558
  • Lynott, D., & Connell, L. (2013). Modality exclusivity norms for 400 nouns: The relationship between perceptual experience and surface word form. Behavior Research Methods, 45(2), 516–526. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0267-0
  • Lynott, D., Corker, K. S., Connell, L., & O’Brien, K. S. (2017). The effect of haptic and ambient temperature experience on prosocial behavior. Archives of Scientific Psychology, 5, 10–18. doi: 10.1037/arc0000031
  • Lynott, D., Corker, K. S., Wortman, J., Connell, L., Donnellan, M. B., & Lucas, R. E. (2014). Replication of ‘“experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth”’ by Williams and Bargh. Social Psychology, 45(7), 216–222. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000187
  • Lyons, I. M., Mattarella-Micke, A., Cieslak, M., Nusbaum, H. C., Small, S. L., & Beilock, S. L. (2010). The role of personal experience in the neural processing of action-related language. Brain and Language, 112(3), 214–222. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.05.006
  • Macaluso, E., Frith, C. D., & Driver, J. (2000). Modulation of human visual cortex by crossmodal spatial attention. Science, 289(5482), 1206–1208. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5482.1206
  • Magee, H. (2009). Talking about taste: How the description of food means and does (BA thesis) Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/10066/10174.
  • Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1–3), 59–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.004
  • Mainland, J., & Sobel, N. (2006). The sniff is part of the olfactory percept. Chemical Senses, 31(2), 181–196. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj012
  • Majid, A., & Burenhult, N. (2014). Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition, 130(2), 266–270. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.004
  • Majid, A., Burenhult, N., Stensmyr, M. C., de Valk, J. M., & Hansson, B. S. (2018). Olfactory language and abstraction across cultures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170139. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0139
  • Majid, A., & Kruspe, N. (2018). Hunter-gatherer olfaction is special. Current Biology, 28(3), 409–413.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.014
  • Majid, A., & Levinson, S. C. (2008). Language does provide support for basic tastes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(01), 86–87. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08003476
  • Majid, A., & Levinson, S. C. (2011). The senses in language and culture. The Senses and Society, 6(1), 5–18. doi: 10.2752/174589311X12893982233551
  • Majid, A., Roberts, S. G., Cilissen, L., Emmorey, K., Nicodemus, B., Woll, B., … Levinson, S. C. (2018). Differential coding of perception in the world’s languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
  • Majid, A., Speed, L. J., Croijmans, I., & Arshamian, A. (2017). What makes a better smeller? Perception, 46(3–4), 406–430. doi: 10.1177/0301006616688224
  • Martin, A., Haxby, J. V., Lalonde, F. o. M., Wiggs, C. L., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science, 270(5233), 102–105. doi: 10.1126/science.270.5233.102
  • Meier, B. P., Moeller, S. K., Riemer-Peltz, M., & Robinson, M. D. (2012). Sweet taste preferences and experiences predict prosocial inferences, personalities, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(1), 163–174. doi: 10.1037/a0025253
  • Melzack, R. (1975). The McGill pain Questionnaire: Major properties and scoring methods. Pain, 1(3), 277–299. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(75)90044-5
  • Meteyard, L., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2007). Motion detection and motion verbs: Language affects low-level visual perception. Psychological Science, 18(11), 1007–1013. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02016.x
  • Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48(7), 788–804. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.002
  • Meteyard, L., Zokaei, N., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2008). Visual motion interferes with lexical decision on motion words. Current Biology, 18(17), R732–R733. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.016
  • Morey, R. D., Glenberg, A. M., Kaschak, M. P., Lakens, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (2018). Testing the ACE effect. A pre-registered, multi-lab test of the Action-Sentence Compatibility Effect. Presented at the Annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans.
  • Mukherjee, S., Kramer, T., & Kulow, K. (2017). The effect of spicy gustatory sensations on variety-seeking. Psychology & Marketing, 34(8), 786–794. doi: 10.1002/mar.21022
  • Myers, C. S. (1904). The taste-names of primitive peoples. British Journal of Psychology, 1(2), 117.
  • Nakagawa, H. (2012). The Importance of TASTE Verbs in Some Khoe Languages, 50(3), 395–420.
  • Nikendei, C., Dengler, W., Wiedemann, G., & Pauli, P. (2005). Selective processing of pain-related word stimuli in subclinical depression as indicated by event-related brain potentials. Biological Psychology, 70(1), 52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.012
  • Olofsson, J. K., Bowman, N. E., & Gottfried, J. A. (2013). High and low roads to odor valence? A choice response-time study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1205–1211. doi: 10.1037/a0033682
  • Olofsson, J. K., Bowman, N. E., Khatibi, K., & Gottfried, J. A. (2012). A time-based account of the perception of odor objects and valences. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1224–1232. doi: 10.1177/0956797612441951
  • Olofsson, J. K., & Gottfried, J. A. (2015). The muted sense: Neurocognitive limitations of olfactory language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(6), 314–321. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.007
  • O’Mahony, M., Goldenberg, M., Stedmon, J., & Alford, J. (1979). Confusion in the use of the taste adjectives “sour” and “bitter.”. Chemical Senses, 4(4), 301–318. doi: 10.1093/chemse/4.4.301
  • O’Meara, C., & Majid, A. (2016). How changing lifestyles impact Seri smellscapes and smell language. Anthropological Linguistics, 58(2), 107–131. doi: 10.1353/anl.2016.0024
  • O'Meara, C., Speed, L. J., San Roque, L., & Majid, A. (2019). Perception metaphors: A view from diversity. In Perception metaphors (pp. 1–16). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
  • Osaka, N., Osaka, M., Morishita, M., Kondo, H., & Fukuyama, H. (2004). A word expressing affective pain activates the anterior cingulate cortex in the human brain: An fMRI study. Behavioural Brain Research, 153(1), 123–127. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.11.013
  • Osawa, Y., & Ellen, R. (2014). The cultural cognition of taste term conflation. The Senses and Society, 9(1), 72–91. doi: 10.2752/174589314X13834112761083
  • Ostarek, M., Joosen, D., Ishag, A., de Nijs, M., & Huettig, F. (2019). Are visual processes causally involved in “perceptual simulation” effects in the sentence-picture verification task? Cognition, 182, 84–94. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.017
  • Ostarek, M., & Vigliocco, G. (2017). Reading sky and seeing a cloud: On the relevance of events for perceptual simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(4), 579–590. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000318
  • Papies, E. K. (2013). Tempting food words activate eating simulations. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00838
  • Pecher, D., van Dantzig, S., & Schifferstein, H. N. J. (2009). Concepts are not represented by conscious imagery. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(5), 914–919. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.5.914
  • Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Verifying different-modality properties for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science, 14(2), 119–124. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01429
  • Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: From a world of experience to the experience of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130300–20130300. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0300
  • Peyron, R., Laurent, B., & García-Larrea, L. (2000). Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000). Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology, 30(5), 263–288. doi: 10.1016/S0987-7053(00)00227-6
  • Plailly, J., Delon-Martin, C., & Royet, J.-P. (2012). Experience induces functional reorganization in brain regions involved in odor imagery in perfumers. Human Brain Mapping, 33(1), 224–234. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21207
  • Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: From reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron, 72(5), 692–697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.001
  • Poldrack, R. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.004
  • Pomp, J., Bestgen, A.-K., Schulze, P., Müller, C. J., Citron, F. M. M., Suchan, B., & Kuchinke, L. (2018). Lexical olfaction recruits olfactory orbitofrontal cortex in metaphorical and literal contexts. Brain and Language, 179, 11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.02.001
  • Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), 458–470. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004
  • Pulvermuller, F., & Hauk, O. (2006). Category-specific conceptual processing of color and form in left fronto-temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16(8), 1193–1201. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj060
  • Ren, D., Tan, K., Arriaga, X. B., & Chan, K. Q. (2015). Sweet love: The effects of sweet taste experience on romantic perceptions. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 32(7), 905–921. doi: 10.1177/0265407514554512
  • Reuter, K., Werning, M., Kuchinke, L., & Cosentino, E. (2017). Reading words hurts: The impact of pain sensitivity on people’s ratings of pain-related words. Language and Cognition, 9(03), 553–567. doi: 10.1017/langcog.2016.29
  • Reznikova, T., Rakhilina, E., & Bonch-Osmolovskaya, A. (2012). Towards a typology of pain predicates. Linguistics, 50(3), doi: 10.1515/ling-2012-0015
  • Richter, M., Eck, J., Straube, T., Miltner, W. H. R., & Weiss, T. (2010). Do words hurt? Brain Activation During the Processing of Pain-Related Words: Pain, 148(2), 198–205. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.08.009.
  • Richter, M., Schroeter, C., Puensch, T., Straube, T., Hecht, H., Ritter, A., … Weiss, T. (2014). Pain-related and negative semantic priming enhances perceived pain intensity. Pain Research and Management, 19(2), 69–74. doi: 10.1155/2014/425321
  • Ritter, A., Franz, M., Puta, C., Dietrich, C., Miltner, W., & Weiss, T. (2016). Enhanced brain responses to pain-related words in chronic Back pain patients and their Modulation by current pain. Healthcare, 4(3), 54. doi: 10.3390/healthcare4030054
  • Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 11–29. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00277-X
  • Rowbotham, S., Holler, J., Lloyd, D., & Wearden, A. (2012). How do we communicate about pain? A systematic analysis of the semantic contribution of co-speech gestures in pain-focused conversations. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 36(1), 1–21. doi: 10.1007/s10919-011-0122-5
  • Royet, J.-P., Plailly, J., Delon-Martin, C., Kareken, D. A., & Segebarth, C. (2003). fMRI of emotional responses to odors: Influence of hedonic valence and judgment, handedness, and gender. NeuroImage, 20(2), 713–728. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00388-4
  • Rueschemeyer, S.-A., Pfeiffer, C., & Bekkering, H. (2010). Body schematics: On the role of the body schema in embodied lexical–semantic representations. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.019
  • Sakamoto, M., & Watanabe, J. (2016). Cross-modal associations between sounds and drink tastes/textures: A study with spontaneous production of sound-symbolic words. Chemical Senses, 41(3), 197–203. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjv078
  • San Roque, L., Kendrick, K. H., Norcliffe, E. J., Brown, P., Defina, R., Dingemanse, M., … Majid, A. (2015). Vision verbs dominate in conversation across cultures, but the ranking of non-visual verbs varies. Cognitive Linguistics, 26, 31–60. doi: 10.1515/cog-2014-0089
  • Schaefer, M., Cherkasskiy, L., Denke, C., Spies, C., Song, H., Malahy, S., … Bargh, J. A. (2018). Incidental haptic sensations influence judgment of crimes. Scientific Reports, 8(1), doi:10.1038/s41598-018-23586-x.
  • Schaefer, M., Denke, C., Heinze, H.-J., & Rotte, M. (2014). Rough primes and rough conversations: Evidence for a modality-specific basis to mental metaphors. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(11), 1653–1659. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst163
  • Semin, G. R., & de Groot, J. H. (2013). The chemical bases of human sociality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1364661313001174
  • Sezille, C., Fournel, A., Rouby, C., Rinck, F., & Bensafi, M. (2014). Hedonic appreciation and verbal description of pleasant and unpleasant odors in untrained, trainee cooks, flavorists and perfumers. Frontiers in Cognitive Science, 5, 12. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00012.
  • Shepherd, G. M. (2004). The human sense of smell: Are we better than we think? PLoS Biology, 2(5), e146. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020146
  • Shepherd, G. M. (2006). Smell images and the flavour system in the human brain. Nature, 444(7117), 316–321. doi: 10.1038/nature05405
  • Si, K., & Jiang, Y. (2017). Bidirectional contrast effects between taste perception and simulation: A simulation-induced adaptation mechanism. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 27(1), 49–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2016.04.002
  • Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L. W. (2005). Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 15(10), 1602–1608. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi038
  • Simmons, W. K., Ramjee, V., Beauchamp, M. S., McRae, K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L. W. (2007). A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color. Neuropsychologia, 45(12), 2802–2810. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.002
  • Smeets, P. A. M., Dagher, A., Hare, T. A., Kullmann, S., van der Laan, L. N., Poldrack, R. A., … Veldhuizen, M. G. (2019). Good practice in food-related neuroimaging. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 109(3), 491–503. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy344
  • Sobel, N., Prabhakaran, V., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., Goode, R. L., Sullivan, E. V., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Sniffing and smelling: Separate subsystems in the human olfactory cortex. Nature, 392(6673), 282–286. doi: 10.1038/32654
  • Sorabji, R. (1971). Aristotle on demarcating the five senses. The Philosophical Review, 80(1), 55–79. doi: 10.2307/2184311
  • Speed, L. J., & Majid, A. (2017a). Dutch modality exclusivity norms: Simulating perceptual modality in space. Behavior Research Methods, 49(6), 2204–2218. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0852-3
  • Speed, L. J., & Majid, A. (2018a). An exception to mental simulation: No evidence for embodied odor language. Cognitive Science, 42(4), 1146–1178. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12593
  • Speed, L. J., & Majid, A. (2018b). Superior olfactory language and cognition in odor-color synaesthesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(3), 468–481.
  • Speed, L. J., van Dam, W. O., Hirath, P., Vigliocco, G., & Desai, R. H. (2017). Impaired comprehension of speed verbs in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1–9. doi:10.1017/S1355617717000248.
  • Speed, L. J., van Dam, W. O, Vigliocco, G., & Desai, R. H. (2018). Movement speed affects speed language comprehension. Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference of the Cognitve Science Society. Presented at the Wisconsin, MA. Wisconsin, MA: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Speed, L. J., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). Eye movements reveal the dynamic simulation of speed in language. Cognitive Science, 38(2), 367–382. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12096
  • Spence, C., Levitan, C. A., Shankar, M. U., & Zampini, M. (2010). Does food color influence taste and flavor perception in humans? Chemosensory Perception, 3(1), 68–84. doi: 10.1007/s12078-010-9067-z
  • Spence, C., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Driver, J. (2001a). The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(2), 330–336. doi: 10.3758/BF03194473
  • Spence, C., Shore, D., & Klein, R. (2001b). Multisensory prior entry.. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 130(4), 799–832. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.799
  • Spence, C., Smith, B., & Auvray, M. (2015). Confusing tastes and flavours. In D. Stokes, M. Matthen, & S. Biggs (Eds.), Perception and its modalities (pp. 247–274). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Stevens, S. S. (1975). Psychophysics. New York: Wiley.
  • Stokes, D., & Biggs, S. (2014). The dominance of the visual. In D. Stokes, M. Matthen, & S. Biggs (Eds.), Perception and Its modalities (pp. 350–378). doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199832798.003.0015.
  • Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, C. M., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., & Fazio, F. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(2), 273–281. doi: 10.1162/0898929053124965
  • Tsakiris, M., & De Preester, H. (2018). The interoceptive mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2004). Action priming by briefly presented objects. Acta Psychologica, 116(2), 185–203. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.01.004
  • Turner, B. O., Paul, E. J., Miller, M. B., & Barbey, A. K. (2018). Small sample sizes reduce the replicability of task-based fMRI studies. Communications Biology, 1(1), doi: 10.1038/s42003-018-0073-z
  • Turnwald, B. P., Boles, D. Z., & Crum, A. J. (2017). Association between indulgent descriptions and vegetable consumption: Twisted carrots and dynamite beets. JAMA Internal Medicine, 177(8), 1216. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2017.1637
  • van Ackeren, M. J., Casasanto, D., Bekkering, H., Hagoort, P., & Rueschemeyer, S.-A. (2012). Pragmatics in action: Indirect requests engage theory of mind areas and the cortical motor network. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(11), 2237–2247. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00274
  • van Ackeren, M. J., & Rueschemeyer, S.-A. (2014). Cross-modal integration of lexical-semantic features during word processing: Evidence from oscillatory dynamics during EEG. PLoS ONE, 9(7), e101042. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101042
  • van Ackeren, M. J., Schneider, T. R., Musch, K., & Rueschemeyer, S.-A. (2014). Oscillatory neuronal activity reflects lexical-semantic feature integration within and across sensory modalities in distributed cortical networks. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(43), 14318–14323. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0958-14.2014
  • van Dam, W. O., van Dijk, M., Bekkering, H., & Rueschemeyer, S.-A. (2012). Flexibility in embodied lexical-semantic representations. Human Brain Mapping, 33(10), 2322–2333. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21365
  • van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. (2008). Perceptual processing affects conceptual processing. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 32(3), 579–590. doi: 10.1080/03640210802035365
  • Veldhuizen, M. G., Bender, G., Constable, R. T., & Small, D. M. (2007). Trying to detect taste in a tasteless solution: Modulation of early gustatory cortex by attention to taste. Chemical Senses, 32(6), 569–581. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjm025
  • Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S.-T., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., Devlin, J. T., & Cappa, S. F. (2014). The neural representation of abstract words: The role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 24(7), 1767–1777. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht025
  • Vigliocco, G., Meteyard, L., Andrews, M., & Kousta, S. (2009). Toward a theory of semantic representation. Language and Cognition, 1(02), 219–247. doi: 10.1515/LANGCOG.2009.011
  • Vukovic, N., Fardo, F., & Shtyrov, Y. (2019). When words burn – language processing differentially modulates pain perception in typical and chronic pain populations. Language and Cognition, 1–15. doi:10.1017/langcog.2018.22.
  • Wang, X., Geng, L., Qin, J., & Yao, S. (2016). The potential relationship between spicy taste and risk seeking. Judgment and Decision Making, 11(6), 547–553.
  • Weiss, T., Miltner, W. H. R., & Dillmann, J. (2003). The influence of semantic priming on event-related potentials to painful laser-heat stimuli in migraine patients. Neuroscience Letters, 340(2), 135–138. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00103-4
  • Wheatley, T., Weisberg, J., Beauchamp, M. S., & Martin, A. (2005). Automatic priming of semantically related words reduces activity in the fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(12), 1871–1885. doi: 10.1162/089892905775008689
  • Wheeler, K. B., & Bergen, B. K. (2010). Meaning in the palm of your hand. In Empirical and experimental methods in conceptual structure, Discourse, and language. Stanford: CSLI.
  • Wierzbicka, A. (2012). Is pain a human universal? A cross-linguistic and cross-cultural perspective on pain. Emotion Review, 4(3), 307–317. doi: 10.1177/1754073912439761
  • Willems, R. M., Hagoort, P., & Casasanto, D. (2010). Body-specific representations of action verbs: Neural evidence from right- and left-handers. Psychological Science, 21(1), 67–74. doi: 10.1177/0956797609354072
  • Willems, R. M., Toni, I., Hagoort, P., & Casasanto, D. (2010). Neural dissociations between action verb understanding and motor imagery. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(10), 2387–2400. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21386
  • Willems, R. M., & van Gerven, M. A. J. (2018). New fMRI methods for the study of language. In Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 975–991). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322(5901), 606–607. doi: 10.1126/science.1162548
  • Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Henriques, A., Barsalou, L. W., & Barrett, L. F. (2019). Primary interoceptive cortex activity during simulated experiences of thebody. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(2), 221–235. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01346
  • Winter, B. (2016). Taste and smell words form an affectively loaded and emotionally flexible part of the English lexicon. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(8), 975–988. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1193619
  • Winter, B., Perlman, M., & Majid, A. (2018). Vision dominates in perceptual language: English sensory vocabulary is optimized for usage. Cognition, 179, 213–220. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.008
  • Winter, B., Perlman, M., Perry, L. K., & Lupyan, G. (2017). Which words are most iconic? Iconicity in English sensory words. Interaction Studies, 18(3), 443–464. doi: 10.1075/is.18.3.07win
  • Wnuk, E., de Valk, J. M., Huisman, J. L. A., & Majid, A. (2017). Hot and cold smells: Odor-temperature associations across cultures. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01373
  • Wnuk, E., & Majid, A. (2014). Revisiting the limits of language: The odor lexicon of Maniq. Cognition, 131(1), 125–138. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.008
  • Wortman, J., Donnellan, M. B., & Lucas, R. E. (2014). Can physical warmth (or coldness) predict trait loneliness? A replication of Bargh and Shalev (2012). Archives of Scientific Psychology, 2(1), 13–19. doi: 10.1037/arc0000007
  • Xie, J., Lu, Z., Wang, R., & Cai, Z. G. (2016). Remember hard but think softly: Metaphorical effects of hardness/softness on cognitive functions. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1343. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01343
  • Xu, J. (2017). “That’s bitter!” culture specific effects of gustatory experience on judgments of fairness and advancement (Masters dissertation). Winnipeg: University of Manitoba.
  • Yaxley, R. H., & Zwaan, R. A. (2007). Simulating visibility during language comprehension. Cognition, 105(1), 229–236. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.003
  • Yee, E., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2016). Putting concepts into context. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1015–1027. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0948-7
  • Yeomans, M. R., Chambers, L., Blumenthal, H., & Blake, A. (2008). The role of expectancy in sensory and hedonic evaluation: The case of smoked salmon ice-cream. Food Quality and Preference, 19(6), 565–573. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2008.02.009
  • Yeshurun, Y., & Sobel, N. (2010). An odor is not worth a thousand words: From multidimensional odors to unidimensional odor objects. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 219–241. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163639
  • Zarzo, M. (2008). Relevant psychological dimensions in the perceptual space of perfumery odors. Food Quality and Preference, 19(3), 315–322. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.10.007
  • Zelano, C., Mohanty, A., & Gottfried, J. A. (2011). Olfactory predictive codes and stimulus templates in piriform cortex. Neuron, 72(1), 178–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.010
  • Zwaan, R. A. (2003). The Immersed Experiencer: Toward An embodied Theory Of language comprehension. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 35–62. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44002-4
  • Zwaan, R. A. (2014). Embodiment and language comprehension: Reframing the discussion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(5), 229–234. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.008
  • Zwaan, R. A., Madden, C. J., Yaxley, R. H., & Aveyard, M. (2004). Moving words: Dynamic representations in language comprehension. Cognitive Science, 28(4), 611–619. doi:10.1016/j.cogsci.2004.03.004.
  • Zwaan, R. A., & Pecher, D. (2012). Revisiting mental simulation in language comprehension: Six replication attempts. PLOS ONE, 7(12), e51382. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051382
  • Zwaan, R. A., Pecher, D., Paolacci, G., Bouwmeester, S., Verkoeijen, P., Dijkstra, K., & Zeelenberg, R. (2017). Participant Nonnaiveté and the reproducibility of cognitive psychology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 1968–1972. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1348-y
  • Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2002). Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science, 13(2), 168–171. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00430
  • Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 1–11. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.1

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.