3,087
Views
49
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Toward a standard model of consciousness: Reconciling the attention schema, global workspace, higher-order thought, and illusionist theories

, , &
Pages 155-172 | Received 12 Mar 2019, Accepted 16 Sep 2019, Published online: 26 Sep 2019

References

  • Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2006). Shifts of visuospatial attention to invisible (metacontrast-masked) singletons: Clues from reaction times and event-related potentials. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, 61–76. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0045-9
  • Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Baars, B. J., & Franklin, S. (2007). An architectural model of conscious and unconscious brain functions: Global workspace theory and IDA. Neural Networks, 20, 955–961. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2007.09.013
  • Bailey, M. D. (2017). Fearful spirits, reasoned follies: The boundaries of superstition in late medieval Europe. Cornell: Cornell University Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
  • Beck, D. M., & Kastner, S. (2009). Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain. Vision Research, 49, 1154–1165. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.012
  • Benassi, V. A., Sweeney, P. D., & Drevno, G. E. (1979). Mind over matter: Perceived success at psychokinesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1377–1386. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.8.1377
  • Blackmore, S. J. (2003). Consciousness in meme machines. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 19–30.
  • Bonda, E., Petrides, M., Frey, S., & Evans, A. (1995). Neural correlates of mental transformations of the body-in-space. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U. S. A., 92, 11180–11184. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.24.11180
  • Borji, A., & Itti, L. (2013). State-of-the-art in visual attention modeling. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 35, 185–207. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2012.89
  • Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature, 391, 756. doi: 10.1038/35784
  • Bzdok, D., Langner, R., Schilbach, L., Jakobs, O., Roski, C., Caspers, S., … Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Characterization of the temporo-parietal junction by combining data-driven parcellation, complementary connectivity analyses, and functional decoding. Neuroimage, 81, 381–392. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.046
  • Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., Keane, J., Scott, S. K., Owen, A. M., Christoffels, I., & Young, A. W. (2002). Reading the mind from eye gaze. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1129–1138. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00008-8
  • Camacho, E. F., & Bordons Alba, C. (2004). Model predictive control. New York, NY: Springer Publishing.
  • Carruthers, G. (2012). A metacognitive model of the sense of agency over thoughts. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 17, 291–314. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2011.627275
  • Chalmers, D. (1995). Facing up to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2, 200–219.
  • Chalmers, D. (2018). The meta-problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 25, 6–61.
  • Conant, R. C., & Ashby, W. R. (1970). Every good regulator of a system must be a model of that system. International Journal of Systems Science, 1, 89–97. doi: 10.1080/00207727008920220
  • Coover, J. E. (1913). “The feeling of being stared at”: Experimental. The American Journal of Psychology, 24, 570–575. doi: 10.2307/1413454
  • Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., & Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 292–297. doi: 10.1038/73009
  • Deco, G., & Rolls, E. T. (2004). A neurodynamical cortical model of visual attention and invariant object recognition. Vision Research, 44, 621–642. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.09.037
  • Dehaene, S. (2014). Consciousness and the brain. New York: Viking Press.
  • Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J.-P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 70, 200–227. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018
  • Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. New York: Little-Brown.
  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
  • de Vignemont, F. (2018). Mind the body: An exploration of bodily self-awareness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Drew, T., Võ, M. L., & Wolfe, J. M. (2013). The invisible gorilla strikes again: Sustained inattentional blindness in expert observers. Psychological Science, 24, 1848–1853. doi: 10.1177/0956797613479386
  • Dundes, A. (1981). The evil eye: A folklore casebook. New York: Garland Press.
  • Francis, B. A., & Wonham, W. M. (1976). The internal model principle of control theory. Automatica, 12, 457–465. doi: 10.1016/0005-1098(76)90006-6
  • Frankish, K. (2016). Illusionism as a theory of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 23, 1–39.
  • Franklin, B., Majault, M. J., Le Roy, J. B., Sallin, C. L., Bailly, J.-S., d’Arcet, J., … Lavoisier, A. (2002). Report of the commissioners charged by the King with the examination of animal magnetism. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 50, 332–363. doi: 10.1080/00207140208410109
  • Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 5, 490–495. doi: 10.3758/BF03208827
  • Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 694–724. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.694
  • Frith, C. (2002). Attention to action and awareness of other minds. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 481–487. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00022-3
  • Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 358, 459–473. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218
  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (1970). The bisected brain. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.
  • Gennaro, R. (2012). The consciousness paradox: Consciousness, concepts, and higher-order thoughts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Graziano, M. S. A. (2013). Consciousness and the social brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Graziano, M. S. A. (2016). Consciousness engineered. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 23, 98–115.
  • Graziano, M. S. A. (2019a). Rethinking consciousness: A scientific theory of subjective experience. New York: Norton.
  • Graziano, M. S. A. (2019b). Attributing awareness to others: The attention schema theory and its relationship to behavioral prediction. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 26, 17–37.
  • Graziano, M. S. A., & Botvinick, M. M. (2002). How the brain represents the body: Insights from neurophysiology and psychology. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX (pp. 136–157). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Graziano, M. S. A., & Kastner, S. (2011). Human consciousness and its relationship to social neuroscience: A novel hypothesis. Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 98–113. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2011.565121
  • Gross, C. G. (1999). The fire that comes from the eye. The Neuroscientist, 5, 58–64. doi: 10.1177/107385849900500108
  • Guterstam, A., Kean, H. H., Webb, T. W., Kean, F. S., & Graziano, M. S. A. (2018). Implicit model of other people’s visual attention as an invisible, force-carrying beam projecting from the eyes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U. S. A., 116, 328–333. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1816581115
  • Head, H., & Holmes, H. G. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain, 34, 102–254. doi: 10.1093/brain/34.2-3.102
  • Holland, O., & Goodman, R. (2003). Robots with internal models: A route to machine consciousness? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 77–109.
  • Holmes, N., & Spence, C. (2004). The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space. Cognitive Processing, 5, 94–105. doi: 10.1007/s10339-004-0013-3
  • Hsieh, P., Colas, J. T., & Kanwisher, N. (2011). Unconscious pop-out: Attentional capture by unseen feature singletons only when top-down attention is available. Psychological Science, 22, 1220–1226. doi: 10.1177/0956797611419302
  • Igelström, K., & Graziano, M. S. A. (2017). The inferior parietal lobe and temporoparietal junction: A network perspective. Neuropsychologia, 105, 70–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.001
  • Igelström, K., Webb, T. W., Kelly, Y. T., & Graziano, M. S. A. (2016). Topographical organization of attentional, social and memory processes in the human temporoparietal cortex. eNeuro, 3, doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0060-16.2016
  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Henry Holt.
  • Jiang, Y., Costello, P., Fang, F., Huang, M., & He, S. (2006). A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U. S. A., 103, 17048–17052. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605678103
  • Kelly, Y. T., Webb, T. W., Meier, J. D., Arcaro, M. J., & Graziano, M. S. A. (2014). Attributing awareness to oneself and to others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U. S. A., 111, 5012–5017. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1401201111
  • Kentridge, R. W., Heywood, C. A., & Weiskrantz, L. (1999). Attention without awareness in blindsight. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 266, 1805–1811. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0850
  • Kentridge, R. W., Heywood, C. A., & Weiskrantz, L. (2004). Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight. Neuropsychologia, 42, 831–835. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.001
  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Mesmer, the Franklin commission, and hypnosis: A counterfactual essay. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 50, 407–419. doi: 10.1080/00207140208410114
  • Lackner, J. R. (1988). Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation. Brain, 111, 281–297. doi: 10.1093/brain/111.2.281
  • Lambert, A., Naikar, N., McLachlan, K., & Aitken, V. (1999). A new component of visual orienting: Implicit effects of peripheral information and subthreshold cues on covert attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 25, 321–340. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.2.321
  • Lau, H., & Rosenthal, D. (2011). Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 365–373. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.009
  • Le Meur, O., Le Callet, P., & Barba, D. (2006). A coherent computational approach to model the bottom-up visual attention. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 28, 802–817. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2006.86
  • Mack, A., & Rock, I. (2000). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Mazzoni, P., & Krakauer, J. W. (2006). An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 3642–3645. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5317-05.2006
  • McCormick, P. A. (1997). Orienting attention without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 23, 168–180. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.23.1.168
  • Medina, J., & Coslett, H. B. (2016). What can errors tell us about body representations? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 33, 5–25. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1188065
  • Metzinger, T. (2009). The ego tunnel: The science of the mind and the Myth of the self. New York: Basic Books.
  • Molenberghs, P., Johnson, H., Henry, J. D., & Mattingley, J. B. (2016). Understanding the minds of others: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 65, 276–291. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.020
  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 83, 435–450. doi: 10.2307/2183914
  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know - verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231
  • Norbre, K., & Kaster, S. (2014). The oxford handbook of attention. New York: Oxford Univeristy Press.
  • Odegaard, B., Knight, R. T., & Lau, H. (2017). Should a few null findings falsify prefrontal theories of conscious perception? The Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 9593–9602. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3217-16.2017
  • Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformations of one’s hands and feet. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 178–241. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90011-9
  • Pattie, F. A. (1994). Mesmer and animal magnetism: A chapter in the history of medicine. Hamilton, NY: Edmonston Publishing.
  • Pesquita, A., Chapman, C. S., & Enns, J. T. (2016). Humans are sensitive to attention control when predicting others’ actions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U. S. A., 113, 8669–8674. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601872113
  • Pettitt, P. (2010). The paleolithic origins of human burial. New York: Routledge.
  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512
  • Prinz, W. (2017). Modeling self on others: An import theory of subjectivity and selfhood. Consciousness and Cognition, 49, 347–362. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.01.020
  • Pullman, P. (2000). The amber spyglass. New York: Scholastic.
  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Rogers-Ramachandran, D. (2000). Phantom limbs and neural plasticity. Archives of Neurology, 57, 317–320. doi: 10.1001/archneur.57.3.317
  • Reynolds, J. H., & Heeger, D. J. (2009). The normalization model of attention. Neuron, 61, 168–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.002
  • Rosenthal, D. (1991). The nature of mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Rosenthal, D. (2005). Consciousness and mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Rowling, J. K. (1999). Harry Potter and the Prisoner of Azkaban. London: Bloomsbury.
  • Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: fMRI investigations of theory of mind. NeuroImage, 19, 1835–1842. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1
  • Saxe, R., & Powell, L. J. (2006). It’s the thought that counts: Specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychological Science, 17, 692–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01768.x
  • Schmidt, K. (2011). Göbekli Tepe: A neolithic site in southwestern Anatolia. In S. R. Steadman & G. McMahon (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of ancient Anatolia (pp. 917–933). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Schwemmer, M. A., Feng, S. F., Holmes, P. J., Gottlieb, J., & Cohen, J. D. (2015). A multi-area stochastic model for a covert visual search task. PLoS One, 10, e0136097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136097
  • Sekiyama, K. (1982). Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands. Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 89–95. doi: 10.3758/BF03204268
  • Shadmehr, R., & Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. (1994). Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 3208–3224. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-03208.1994
  • Shulman, G. L., Pope, D. L., Astafiev, S. V., McAvoy, M. P., Snyder, A. Z., & Corbetta, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dominance during spatial selective attention and target detection occurs outside the dorsal frontoparietal network. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 3640–3651. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4085-09.2010
  • Sidky, H. (2017). The origins of shamanism, spirit beliefs, and religiosity: A cognitive anthropological perspective. London: Lexington Books.
  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074. doi: 10.1068/p281059
  • Simons, J. S., Garrison, J. R., & Johnson, M. K. (2017). Brain mechanisms of reality monitoring. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 462–473. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.012
  • Taylor, J. A., Krakauer, J. W., & Ivry, R. B. (2014). Explicit and implicit contributions to learning in a sensorimotor adaptation task. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 3023–3032. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3619-13.2014
  • Thoroughman, K. A., & Shadmehr, R. (2000). Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives. Nature, 407, 742–747. doi: 10.1038/35037588
  • Thoroughman, K. A., & Taylor, J. A. (2005). Rapid reshaping of human motor generalization. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 8948–8953. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1771-05.2005
  • Titchner, E. B. (1898). The feeling of being stared at. Science, 8, 895–897. doi: 10.1126/science.8.208.895
  • Tolkien, J. R. R. (1955). The lord of the rings. London: Allen and Unwin.
  • Tsushima, Y., Sasaki, Y., & Watanabe, T. (2006). Greater disruption due to failure of inhibitory control on an ambiguous distractor. Science, 314, 1786–1788. doi: 10.1126/science.1133197
  • Turing, A. M. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind; A Quarterly Review of Psychology and Philosophy, 59, 433–460. doi: 10.1093/mind/LIX.236.433
  • Vallar, G., & Perani, D. (1986). The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions: A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia, 24, 609–622. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90001-1
  • Vallar, G., & Ronchi, R. (2009). Somatoparaphrenia: A body delusion. A review of the neuropsychological literature. Experimental Brain Research, 192, 533–551. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1562-y
  • van den Boogaard, J., Treur, J., & Turpijn, M. (2017). A neurologically inspired neural network model for Graziano’s attention schema theory for consciousness. International Work Conference on the Interplay Between Natural and Artificial Computation: Natural and Artificial Computation for Biomedicine and Neuroscience, 10337, 10–21. Part 1. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-59740-9_2
  • van Vugt, B., Dagnino, B., Vartak, D., Safaai, H., Panzeri, S., & Dehaene, S. (2018). The threshold for conscious report: Signal loss and response bias in visual and frontal cortex. Science, 360, 537–542. doi: 10.1126/science.aar7186
  • Webb, T. W., & Graziano, M. S. A. (2015). The attention schema theory: A mechanistic account of subjective awareness. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00500
  • Webb, T. W., Igelström, K., Schurger, A., & Graziano, M. S. A. (2016a). Cortical networks involved in visual awareness independently of visual attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U. S. A., 113, 13923–13928. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611505113
  • Webb, T. W., Kean, H. H., & Graziano, M. S. A. (2016b). Effects of awareness on the control of attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28, 842–851. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00931
  • Wellman, H. M. (2018). Theory of mind: The state of the art. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 728–755. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2018.1435413
  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
  • Winer, G. A., Cottrell, J. E., Gregg, V., Fournier, J. S., & Bica, L. S. (2002). Fundamentally misunderstanding visual perception: Adults’ belief in visual emissions. American Psychologist, 57, 417–424. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.6-7.417
  • Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., … Buckner, R. L. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.