152
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Commentaries

Conscious contents: Their unanalyzable, arbitrary, and unarbitrary properties

&
Pages 187-189 | Received 14 Nov 2019, Accepted 07 Feb 2020, Published online: 18 Feb 2020

References

  • Block, N. (2007). Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 481–548. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07002786
  • Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and problems of knowledge: The Managua lectures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Desender, K., van Opstal, F. V., & van den Bussche, E. (2014). Feeling the conflict: The crucial role of conflict experience in adaptation. Psychological Science, 25, 675–683. doi: 10.1177/0956797613511468
  • Dreisbach, G., & Fischer, R. (2015). Conflicts as aversive signals for control adaptation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 255–260. doi: 10.1177/0963721415569569
  • Freeman, W. J. (2004). William James on consciousness, revisited. Chaos and Complexity Letters, 1, 17–42.
  • Gray, J. A. (2004). Consciousness: Creeping up on the hard problem. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Liveright Publishing Corporation.
  • Lashley, K. S. (1923). The behavioristic interpretation of consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 30, 237–272. (Part 1), 329–353. doi: 10.1037/h0073839
  • Levitin, D. J. (2007). This is your brain on music: The science of a human obsession. New York: Plume.
  • Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • Livnat, A., & Pippenger, N. (2006). An optimal brain can be composed of conflicting agents. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, 3198–3202. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510932103
  • Loewenstein, G. (2007). Defining affect. Social Science Information, 46, 405–410. doi: 10.1177/05390184070460030106
  • Merker, B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 63–134. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07000891
  • Merker, B. (2013). The efference cascade, consciousness, and its self: Naturalizing the first person pivot of action control. Frontiers in Psycholology, 4, 1–20. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00501
  • Morsella, E., Hoover, M. A., & Bargh, J. A. (2013). Functionalism redux: How adaptive action constrains perception, simulation, and evolved intuitions. In K. L. Johnson, & M. Shiffrar (Eds.), People watching: Social, perceptual, and neurophysiological studies of body perception (pp. 256–282). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83, 435–450. doi: 10.2307/2183914
  • Searle, J. R. (2000). Consciousness. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 23, 557–578. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.557
  • Shallice, T. (1972). Dual functions of consciousness. Psychological Review, 79, 383–393. doi: 10.1037/h0033135

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.