51
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The role of cognitive estimation in understanding the mental states of others

, , &
Pages 381-400 | Received 20 Oct 2023, Accepted 07 May 2024, Published online: 23 May 2024

References

  • Adams, R. B. Jr, Rule, N., Franklin, R. G. Jr, Wang, E., Stevenson, M. T., Yoshikawa, S., Nomura, M., Sato, W., Kveraga, K., & Ambady, N. (2010). Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(1), 97–108. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21187
  • Adenzato, M., Cavallo, M., & Enrici, I. (2010). Theory of mind ability in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: An analysis of the neural cognitive, and social levels. Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 2–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.001
  • Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 693–716. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514
  • Adolphs, R., Baron-Cohen, S., & Tranel, D. (2002). Impaired recognition of social emotions following amygdala damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1264–1274. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902760807258
  • Ainsworth, M., Sallet, J., Joly, O., Kyriazis, D., Kriegeskorte, N., Duncan, J., Schüffelgen, U., Rushworth, M. F. S., & Bell, A. H. (2021). Viewing ambiguous social interactions increases functional connectivity between frontal and temporal nodes of the social brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 41(28), 6070–6086. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0870-20.2021
  • Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7), 267–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01501-1
  • Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(4), 268–277. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1884
  • Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(11), 1032–1037. https://doi.org/10.1038/14833
  • Axelrod, B. N., & Millis, S. R. (1994). Preliminary standardization of the cognitive estimation test. Assessment, 1(3), 269–274. https://doi.org/10.1177/107319119400100307
  • Bach, L., Happé, F., Fleminger, S., & Powell, J. (2000). Theory of mind: Independence of executive function and the role of the frontal cortex in acquired brain injury. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 5(3), 175–192. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800050083520
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory-of-mind. MIT Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A., & Williams, S. C. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11(6), 1891–1898. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00621.x.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J. (2000). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The ‘Reading The Mind In The Eyes’ test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00715
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4(3), 311–331. https://doi.org/10.1080/713756761
  • Bates, E., Wilson, S. M., Saygin, A. P., Dick, F., Sereno, M. I., Knight, R. T., & Dronker, N. F. (2003). Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Nature Neuroscience, 6(5), 448–450. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1050
  • Beer, J. S., Heerey, E. H., Keltner, D., Scabini, D., & Knight, R. T. (2003). The regulatory function of self-conscious emotion: Insights from patients with orbitofrontal damage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(4), 594–604. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.594
  • Beer, J. S., & Ochsner, K. N. (2006). Social cognition: A multi level analysis. Brain Research, 1079(1), 98–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.002
  • Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. Brain, 125(8), 1696–1708. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf190
  • Bibby, H., & McDonald, S. (2005). Theory of mind after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 43(1), 99–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.027
  • Bora, E., & Pantelis, C. (2016). On the relation between theory of mind and executive functioning: A developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. In S. K. Whitbourne (Ed.), The encyclopedia of adulthood and aging (Vol. 1, pp. 1–5). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
  • Borg, J. S., Hynes, C., Van Horn, J., Grafton, S., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2006). Consequences, action, and intention as factors in moral judgments: An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(5), 803–817. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.5.803
  • Bosco, F. M., Parola, A., Sacco, K., Zettin, M., & Angeleri, R. (2017). Communicativepragmatic disorders in traumatic brain injury: The role of theory of mind and executive functions. Brain and Language, 168, 73–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2017.01.007
  • Brand, M., Kalbe, E., Fujiwara, E., Huber, M., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2003). Cognitive estimation in patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease and alcoholic Korsakoff patients. Neuropsychologia, 41(5), 575–584. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00183-5
  • Brown, L., Sherbenou, J. R., & Johnsen, S. K. (1989). Test of nonverbal intelligence: A language-free measure of cognitive ability. Springer International Publishing/Springer Nature.
  • Brüne, M., & Brüne-Cohrs, U. (2006). Theory of mind—evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(4), 437–455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.001
  • Bullard, S. E., Fein, D., Gleeson, M. K., Tischer, N., Mapou, R. L., & Kaplan, E. (2004). The biber cognitive estimation test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(6), 835–846. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2003.12.002
  • Camille, N., Coricelli, G., Sallet, J., Pradat-Diehl, P., Duhamel, J.-R., & Sirigu, A. (2004). The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science, 304(5674), 1167–1170. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1094550
  • Cardillo, E. R., McQuire, M., & Chatterjee, A. (2018). Selective metaphor impairments after left, not right, hemisphere injury. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(2308), 2308. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02308
  • Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73–92. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.298
  • Champagne-Lavau, M., & Joanette, Y. (2009). Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: Different patterns of deficits. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22(5), 413–426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.02.002
  • Channon, S., & Crawford, S. (2000). The effects of anterior lesions on performance on a story comprehension test: Left anterior impairment on a theory of mind-type task. Neuropsychologia, 38(7), 1006–1017. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00154-2
  • Channon, S., Pellijeff, A., & Rule, A. (2005). Social cognition after head injury: Sarcasm and theory of mind. Brain and Language, 93(2), 123–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.09.002
  • Channon, S., Rule, A., Maudgil, D., Martinos, M., Pellijeff, A., Frankl, J., Drury, H., & Shieff, C. (2007). Interpretation of mentalistic actions and sarcastic remarks: Effects of frontal and posterior lesions on mentalising. Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1725–1734. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.021
  • Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Làdavas, E., & di Pellegrino, G. (2007). Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), 84–92. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm001
  • Cipolotti, L., MacPherson, S. E., Gharooni, S., van-Harskamp, N., Shallice, T., Chan, E., & Nachev, P. (2018). Cognitive estimation: Performance of patients with focal frontal and posterior lesions. Neuropsychologia, 115, 70–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.08.017
  • Cohen-Zimmerman, C., Khilwani, H., Smith, G. N. L., Krueger, F., Gordon, B., & Grafman, J. (2021). The neural basis for mental state attribution: A voxel-based lesion mapping study. Human Brain Mapping, 42(1), 65–79. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25203
  • Cole, S. N., Smith, D. M., Ragan, K., Suurmond, R., & Armitage, C. J. (2021). Synthesizing the effects of mental simulation on behavior change: Systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(5), 1514–1537. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-01880-6
  • Corcoran, R. (2000). Theory of mind in other clinical conditions: Is a selective -theory of mind-deficit exclusive to autism? In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 391–421). Oxford Univ. Press.
  • Corcoran, R., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Autobiographical memory and theory of mind: Evidence of a relationship in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 33(5), 897–905. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291703007529
  • Cordonier, N., Fossard, M., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2020). Differential impairments in irony comprehension in brain-damaged individuals: Insight from contextual processing, theory of mind, and executive functions. Neuropsychology, 34(7), 750–763. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000682
  • Costa-Cordella, S., Arevalo-Romero, C., Parada, F. J., & Rossi, A. (2021). Social support and cognition: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 637060. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.637060
  • Cuzzolin, F., Morelli, A., Cîrstea, B., & Sahakian, B. J. (2020). Knowing me, knowing you: Theory of mind in AI. Psychological Medicine, 50(7), 1057–1061. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720000835
  • Dal Monte, O., Schintu, S., Pardini, M., Berti, A., Wassermann, E. M., Grafman, J., & Krueger, F. (2014). The left inferior frontal gyrus is crucial for reading the mind in the eyes: Brain lesion evidence. Cortex, 58, 9–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.002
  • Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M., & Damasio, A. R. (1994). The return of Phineas gage: Clues about the brain from a skull of a famous patient. Science, 264(5162), 1102–1105. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.8178168
  • D’Aniello, G. E., Scarpina, F., Albani, G., Castelnuovo, G., & Mauro, A. (2015). Disentangling the relationship between cognitive estimation abilities and executive functions: a study on patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurological Sciences, 36(8), 1425–1429. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-015-2158-5
  • Dashtestani, H., Zaragoza, R., Pirsiavash, H., Knutson, K.M., Kermanian, R., Cui, J., Harrison, J.D., Jr., Halem, M., & Gandjbakhche, A. (2018). Canonical correlation analysis of brain prefrontal activity measured by functional near infra-red spectroscopy (fNIRS) during a moral judgment task. Behavioural Brain Research, 359, 73–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.10.022
  • Davis, H. L., & Pratt, C. (1995). The development of children’s theory of mind: The working memory explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47(1), 25–31. https://doi.org/10.1080/00049539508258765
  • Decety, J. (2004). L’empathie est-elle une simulation mentale de la subjectivité d’autrui? In A. Berthoz (Ed.), L’Empathie (pp. 53–88). Odile Jacob.
  • Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3(2), 71–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187
  • Della Sala, S., MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., Sacco, L., & Spinnler, H. (2004). The role of semantic memory on the Cognitive Estimation Task: Evidence from Alzheimer’s Disease and healthy adult aging. Journal of Neurology, 251(2), 156–164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-004-0292-8
  • Della Sala, S., MacPherson, S., Phillips, L. H., Sacco, L., & Spinnler, H. J. N. S. (2003). How many camels are there in Italy? Cognitive Estimates Standardised on the Italian Population, 24(1), 10–15.
  • Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I., & Pillon, B. (2000). The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology, 55(11), 1621–1626. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621
  • Dvash, J., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2014). Theory of mind and empathy as multidimensional constructs: Neurological foundations. Topics in Language Disorders, 34(4), 282–295. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000040
  • Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00025-7
  • Epley, N., Keysar, B., Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(3), 327–339. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.327
  • Eslinger, P. J. (1998). Neurological and neuropsychological bases of empathy. European Neurology, 39(4), 193–199. https://doi.org/10.1159/000007933
  • Eslinger, P. J., & Damasio, A. R. (1985). Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology, 35(12), 1731–1731. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.35.12.1731
  • Eslinger, P. J., Grattan, L. M., & Damasio, A. R. (1992). Developmental consequences of childhood frontal lobe damage. Archives of Neurology, 49(7), 764–769. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1992.00530310112021
  • Fletcher, P. C., Happé, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition, 57(2), 109–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00692-R
  • Fortune, D. G., & Richards, H. L. (2017). Assessing cognitive estimation and its effects on community integration in people with acquired brain injury undergoing rehabilitation. BioMed Research International, 2017, 2874819. doi:10.1155/2017/2874819
  • Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531–534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001
  • Geraci, A., Surian, L., Ferraro, M., & Cantagallo, A. (2010). Theory of Mind in patients with ventromedial or dorsolateral prefrontal lesions following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 24(7–8), 978–987. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2010.487477
  • Grafman, J. (1994). Alternative frameworks for the conceptualization of prefrontal lobe functions. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 9, pp. XIV-432). Elsevier.
  • Grafman, J., Schwab, K., Warden, D., Pridgen, B. S., Brown, H. R., & Salazar, A. M. (1996). Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: A report of the Vietnam head injury study. Neurology, 46(5), 1231–1238. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.46.5.1231
  • Greene, J. D. (2007). Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian? A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 322–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.004
  • Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389–400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027
  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105–2108. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872
  • Grezes, J., Frith, C. D., & Passingham, R. E. (2004). Brain mechanisms for inferring deceit in the actions of others. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(24), 5500–5505. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0219-04.2004
  • Grossmann, T. (2017). The eyes as windows into other minds: An integrative perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(1), 107–121. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616654457
  • Grossmann, T., Cross, E. S., Ticini, L. F., & Daum, M. M. (2013). Action observation in the infant brain: The role of body form and motion. Social Neuroscience, 8(1), 22–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.696077
  • Han, H. (2017). Neural correlates of moral sensitivity and moral judgment associated with brain circuitries of selfhood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Moral Education, 46(2), 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2016.1262834
  • Han, H., Chen, J., Jeong, C., & Glover, G. H. (2015). Relationship between brain regions associated with self and moral functioning in moral judgment. Human Brain Mapping, 1, 15–18.
  • Happe, A. F., Brownell, H., & Winner, E. (1999). Acquired ‘theory of mind’ impairments following stroke. Cognition, 70(3), 211–240. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00005-0
  • Havet-Thomassin, V., Allain, P., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2006). What about theory of mind after severe brain injury? Brain Injury, 20(1), 83–91. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050500340655
  • Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Crawford, J. R., Ietswaart, M., & Summers, F. (2006). Theory of mind following traumatic brain injury: The role of emotion recognition and executive dysfunction. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1623–1628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.020
  • Hoerl, C. (2018). Episodic memory and theory of mind: A connection reconsidered. Mind & Language, 33(2), 148–160. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12170
  • Hoffman, P., & MacPherson, S. E. (2022). What determines cognitive estimation ability? Changing contributions of semantic and executive domains as a function of age. Journal of Neuropsychology, 16(3), 481–497. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12279
  • Kampe, K. K., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). “Hey John”: Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(12), 5258–5263. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-05258.2003
  • Kawashima, R., Sugiura, M., Kato, T., Nakamura, A., Hatano, K., & Ito, K. (1999). The human amygdala plays an important role in gaze monitoring: A PET study. Brain, 122(4), 779–783. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/122.4.779
  • Kimberg, D. Y., Coslett, H. B., & Schwartz, M. F. (2007). Power in voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(7), 1067–1080. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1067
  • Kleinke, C. L. (1986). Gaze and eye contact: A research review. Psychological Bulletin, 100(1), 78–100. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.100.1.78
  • Koster-Hale, J., Richardson, H., Velez, N., Asaba, M., Young, L., & Saxe, R. (2017). Mentalizing regions represent distributed, continuous, and abstract dimensions of others’ beliefs. NeuroImage, 161(1), 9–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.026
  • Landriscina, F. (2013). Simulation and learning: A model-centered approach. Springer.
  • Lang, B., & Perner, J. (2002). Understanding of intention and false belief and the development of self-control. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 67–76. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151002166325
  • Lee, T. M. C., Alison, K. Y. I., Wang, K., Xi, C. H., Hu, P. P., Mak, H. K. F., Han, S. H., & Chan, C. C. H. (2010). Faux Pas deficits in people with medial frontal lesions as related to impaired understanding of a speaker's mental state. Neuropsychologia, 48(6), 1670–1676. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.012
  • Lockwood, P. L., & Klein-Flügge, M. C. (2021). Computational modelling of social cognition and behaviour-a reinforcement learning primer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 761–771.
  • Lundgren, K., Brownell, H., Roy, S., & Cayer-Meade, C. (2006). A metaphor comprehension intervention for patients with right hemisphere brain damage: A pilot study. Brain and Language, 99(1-2), 69–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.06.044
  • MacPherson, S., Wagner, G., Murphy, P., Bozzali, M., & Cipolotti, L. (2014). Bringing the cognitive estimation task into the 21st century: Normative data on Two New parallel forms. PLoS One, 9(3), e92554. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092554
  • Mah, L. W. Y., Courtney, A. M., & Grafman, J. (2005). Deficits in social knowledge following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 17(1), 66–74. https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.17.1.66
  • Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J., & Baldwin, D. A.2001). Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition. The MIT Press.
  • Mareschal, D., & Thomas, M. S. C. (2007). Computational modeling of social cognition and behavior—A reinforcement learning approach. In P. Haggard, Y. Rossetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition (pp. 263–290). Oxford University Press.
  • Mazza, M., Costagliola, C., Di Michele, V., Magliani, V., Pollice, R., Ricci, A., Giovanbattista, E. D., Roncone, R., Casacchia, M., & Galzio, R. J. (2007). Deficit of social cognition in subjects with surgically treated frontal lobe lesions and in subjects affected by schizophrenia. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 257(1), 12–22. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-006-0676-0
  • Meinhardt-Injac, B., Daum, M. M., Meinhardt, G., & Persike, M. (2018). The two-systems account of theory of mind: Testing the links to social-perceptual and cognitive abilities. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 25. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00025
  • Meyer, M. L., & Collier, E. (2020). Theory of minds: Managing mental state inferences in working memory is associated with the dorsomedial subsystem of the default network and social integration. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(1), 63–73. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa022
  • Milders, M., Fuchs, S., & Crawford, J. R. (2003). Neuropsychological impairments and changes in emotional and social behaviour following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(2), 157–172. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.25.2.157.13642
  • Mitchell, R. L., & Phillips, L. H. (2015). The overlapping relationship between emotion perception and theory of mind. Neuropsychologia, 70, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.018
  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R. D., Eslinger, P. J., Bramati, I. E., Mouräo-Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P. A., & Pessoa, L. (2002). The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(7), 2730–2736. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02730.2002
  • Moreau, N., Viallet, F., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2013). Using memories to understand others: The role of episodic memory in theory of mind impairment in Alzheimer disease. Ageing Research Reviews, 12(4), 833–839. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2013.06.005
  • Morris, R. G., Miotto, E. C., Feigenbaum, J. D., Bullock, P., & Polkey, C. E. (1997). The effect of goal-subgoal conflict on planning ability after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in humans. Neuropsychologia, 35(8), 1147–1157. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00009-2
  • Muller, F., Simion, A., Reviriego, E., Galera, C., Mazaux, J., Barat, M., & Joseph, P. A. (2010). Exploring theory of mind after severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex, 46(9), 1088–1099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.014
  • Muraki, E. J., Speed, L. J., & Pexman, P. M. (2023). Insights into embodied cognition and mental imagery from aphantasia. Nature Reviews Psychology, 2(10), 591–605. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-023-00221-9
  • Mutter, B., Alcorn, M. B., & Welsh, M. (2006). Theory of mind and executive function: Working-memory capacity and inhibitory control as predictors of false-belief task performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102(3), 819–835. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.102.3.819-835
  • Naito, M. (2003). The relationship between theory of mind and episodic memory: Evidence for the development of autonoetic consciousness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(4), 312–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00075-4
  • Oberauer, K. (2005). Executive functions, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind–Does it all come together? In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children's cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 285–299). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Oliver, L. D., Vieira, J. B., Neufeld, R. W. J., Dziobek, I., & Mitchell, D. G. V. (2018). Greater involvement of action simulation mechanisms in emotional vs cognitive empathy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(4), 367–380. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy013
  • Ouerchefani, R., Ouerchefani, N., Ben Rejeb, M. R., & Le Gall, D. (2021a). Impaired perception of unintentional transgression of social norms after prefrontal cortex damage: Relationship to decision making, emotion recognition, and executive functions. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 37(2), 249–273. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acab078
  • Ouerchefani, R., Ouerchefani, N., Ben Rejeb, M. R., & Le Gall, D. (2021b). Impaired social perception of visual cues from eyes and face: Evidence from prefrontal cortex damage. Social Neuroscience, 607–626. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2021.1983458
  • Ouerchefani, R., Ouerchefani, N., Ben Rejeb, M. R., & Le Gall, D. (2022). Neurocognitive and neural mechanisms underlying deficit on the Reading Mind in the Eyes Task: Evidence from patients with focal prefrontal cortex damage. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 44(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2022.2057928
  • Ouerchefani, R., Ouerchefani, N., Ben Rejeb, M. R., & Le Gall, D. (2024). Pragmatic language comprehension: Role of theory of mind, executive functions, and the prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 194. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108756
  • Parente, A., Manfredi, V., Villani, F., Franceschetti, S., & Giovagnoli, A. R. (2013). Investigating higher-order cognitive functions in temporal lobe epilepsy: Cognitive estimation. Epilepsy Behavior, 29(2), 330–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.07.031
  • Pelphrey, K. A., & Morris, J. P. (2006). Brain mechanisms for interpreting the actions of others from biological-motion cues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 136–140. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00423.x
  • Perner, J., Kloo, D., & Gornik, E. (2007). Episodic memory development: Theory of mind is part of re-experiencing experienced events. Infant and Child Development, 16(5), 471–490. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.517
  • Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2000). On the relation between theory of mind and executive functioning: A developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 270–286). Oxford University Press.
  • Perry, R. J., Rosen, H. R., Kramer, J. H., Beer, J. S., Levenson, R. L., & Miller, B. L. (2001). Hemispheric dominance for emotions, empathy and social behaviour: Evidence from right and left handers with frontotemporal dementia. Neurocase, 7(2), 145–160. https://doi.org/10.1093/neucas/7.2.145
  • Piemontesi, S. E. (2010). Procesos en neurociencia social cognitiva y afectiva Para la comprensi’on e interacci’on social: Un Marco integrador. Revista Argentina de Ciencias del Comportamiento, 2(3), 30–44.
  • Raffaelli, Q., Wilcox, R., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2020). The neuroscience of imaginative thought: An integrative framework. In A. Abraham (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the imagination (pp. 332–353). Cambridge University Press.
  • Raine, A., & Yang, Y. (2006). Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(3), 203–213. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl033
  • Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions. Neuroimage, 22(4), 1694–1703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.015
  • Rivière, E., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2019). Which contextual and sociocultural information predict irony perception? Discourse Processes, 56(3), 259–277. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2019.1637204
  • Rorden, C., Karnath, H. O., & Bonilha, L. (2007). Improving lesion-symptom mapping. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(7), 1081–1088. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1081
  • Rowe, A. D., Bullock, P. R., Polkey, C. E., & Morris, R. G. (2001). ‘Theory of mind’ impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excisions. Brain, 124(3), 600–616. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/124.3.600
  • Röska-Hardy, L. (2024). Mental simulation and perspective taking: A developmental perspective. Journal of Cognition and Development, 25(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2023.2237228
  • Rule, N. O., Ambady, N., Adams, R. B., & MacraeJr, C. N. (2008). Facets of the face: Accuracy and awareness in the perception and categorization of male sexual orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1019–1028. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013194
  • Russell, T. A., Rubia, K., Bullmore, E. T., Soni, W., Suckling, J., Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A., Williams, S. C. R., & Sharma, T. (2000). Exploring the social brain in schizophrenia: Left prefrontal underactivation during mental state attribution. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(12), 2040–2042. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.12.2040
  • Sabbagh, M. (2004). Understanding orbitofrontal contributions to theory-of-mind reasoning: Implications for autism. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 209–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2003.04.002
  • Sabbagh, M. A., Moulson, M. C., & Harkness, K. L. (2004). Neural correlates of mental state decoding in human adults: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 415–426. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892904322926755
  • Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., Sawada, R., Kubota, Y., Yoshimura, S., & Toichi, M. (2016). Structural neural substrates of Reading the mind in the eyes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00151
  • Scarpina, F., D'Aniello, G. E., Mauro, A., Castelnuovo, G., & MacPherson, S. E. (2015). How many segments are there in an orange: Normative data for the new Cognitive Estimation Task in an Italian population. Neurological Sciences, 36(10), 1889–1895. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-015-2276-0
  • Scarpina, F., Mauro, A., D’Aniello, G. E., Albani, G., Castelnuovo, G., Ambiel, E., & MacPherson, S. E. (2017). Cognitive estimation in non-demented Parkinson’s disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 32(4), 381–390. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acx019
  • Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 609–612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009
  • Schurz, M., Tholen, M. G., Perner, J., Mars, R. B., & Kanske, P. (2020). The neural correlates of affective and cognitive theory of mind: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, 146(8), 721–744.
  • Shallice, T. (2001). ‘Theory of mind’ and the prefrontal cortex. Brain, 124(2), 247–248. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/124.2.247
  • Shallice, T., & Evans, M. E. (1978). The involvement of the frontal lobes in cognitive estimation. Cortex, 14(2), 294–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(78)80055-0
  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2009). Empathic processing: Its cognitive and affective dimensions and neurological foundations. In J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 215–232). MIT Press.
  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Berger, B. D., Goldsher, D., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2005). Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18(1), 55–67. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnn.0000152228.90129.99
  • Shamay-Tsoory, S., Tomer, R., Berger, B., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2003). Characterization of empathy deficits following prefrontal brain damage: The role of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), 324–337. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903321593063
  • Sheldon, S., Fenerci, C., & Gurguryan, L. (2019). A neurocognitive perspective on the forms and functions of autobiographical memory retrieval. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 13, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2019.00004
  • Shenhav, A., & Greene, J. D. (2010). Moral judgments recruit domain-general valuation mechanisms to integrate representations of probability and magnitude. Neuron, 67(4), 667–677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.07.020
  • Siegal, M., Carrington, J., & Radel, M. (1996). Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain and Language, 53(1), 40–50. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1996.0035
  • Silverberg, N. D., Hanks, R. A., & Mckay, C. (2007). Cognitive estimation in traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(5), 898–902. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617707071135
  • Smith, M. L., & Milner, B. (1984). Differential effects of frontal-lobe lesions on cognitive estimation and spatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 22(6), 697–705. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(84)90096-4
  • Smith, M. L., & Milner, B. (1988). Estimation of frequency of occurrence of abstract designs after frontal or temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia, 26(2), 297–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(88)90082-6
  • Souchay, C., Ohlsson, M., & Zalla, T. (2018). Autobiographical memory and theory of mind in autism spectrum disorder. In J. L. Johnson, G. S. Goodman, & P. C. Mundy (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of memory, autism spectrum disorder, and the law (pp. 92–106). Wiley Blackwell.
  • Sperber, D. (2000). Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 117–137). Oxford University Press.
  • Spreng, R. N. (2013). Examining the role of memory in social cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 437. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00437
  • Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 640–656. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998562942
  • Stuss, D. T., Gallup, G. G. Jr., & Alexander, M. P. (2001). The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’. Brain, 124(2), 279–286. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/124.2.279
  • Stuss, D. T., & Levine, B. (2002). Adult clinical neuropsychology: Lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 401–433. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135220
  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). A reexamination of the theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In T. Charman, N. Yimiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from development and theory (pp. 173–193). Erlbaum.
  • Talairach, J., & Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: Three dimensional proportional system: An approach to medical cerebral imaging. G. Thieme Medical Publishers.
  • Taylor, R., & O’Carroll, R. (1995). Cognitive estimation in neurological disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34(2), 223–228. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01456.x
  • Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: A practical scale. The Lancet, 2(7872), 81–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(74)91639-0
  • Thye, M. D., Donna, L., Murdaugh, D. L., & Kana, R. K. (2018). Brain mechanisms underlying reading the mind from eyes, voice, and actions. Neuroscience, 374, 172–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.01.045
  • Tranel, D., Bechara, A., & Denburg, N. L. (2002). Asymmetric functional roles of right and left ventromedial prefrontal cortices in social conduct, decision-making, and emotional processing. Cortex, 38(4), 589–612. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70024-8
  • Turkstra, L. S. (2008). Conversation-based assessment of social cognition in adults with traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 22(5), 397–409. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050802027059
  • Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., Mazoyer, B., & Joliot, M. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage, 15(1), 273–289. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978
  • Varga, A. C. (1984). Declaration of Helsinki. Adopted by the 18th World Medical Assembly in Helsinki, Finland, and revised by the 29th World Assembly in Tokyo, 1971. In The main issue in bioethics (Rev. ed., pp. 273–289). Paulist Press.
  • Vinette, C., Gosselin, F., & Schyns, P. G. (2004). Spatio-temporal dynamics of face recognition in a flash: It's in the eyes. Cognitive Science, 28(2), 289–301.
  • Wade, M., Prime, H., Jenkins, J. M., Yeates, K. O., Williams, T., & Lee, K. (2018). On the relation between theory of mind and executive functioning: A developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(6), 2119–2140. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1459-0
  • Wagner, G. P., MacPherson, S. E., Parente, M. A. M. P., & Trentini, C. M. (2011). Cognitive estimation abilities in healthy and clinical populations: The use of the Cognitive Estimation Test. Neurological Sciences, 32(2), 203–210. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-010-0449-4
  • Waytz, A., Hershfield, H. E., & Tamir, D. I. (2015). Mental simulation and meaning in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(2), 336–355. doi:10.1037/a0038322
  • Wilf, A., Lee, S. S., Liang, P. P., & Morency, L. P. (2023). Think twice: Perspective-taking improves large language models’ theory-of-mind capabilities. arXiv preprint arXiv:2311.10227.
  • Wolf, R. C., Philippi, C. L., Motzkin, J. C., Baskaya, M. K., & Koenigs, M. (2014). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates visual attention during facial emotion recognition. Brain, 137(6), 1772–1780. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awu063
  • Wolf, R. C., Pujara, M., Baskya, M. K., & Koenings, M. (2016). Emotion recognition deficits associated with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions are improved by gaze manipulation. Cortex, 82, 255–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.06.017
  • Yeh, Z. T., Ming-Cheng, T., Ming-Dar, T., Chiao-Yu, L., & Kaw-Chen, W. (2016). The relationship between theory of mind and the executive functions: Evidence from patients with frontal lobe damage. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2016.1185425
  • Zalla, T., & Korman, J. (2018). Prior knowledge, episodic control and theory of mind in autism: Toward an integrative account of social cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(6), 758. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00752
  • Zebrowitz, L. A. (2006). Finally, faces find favor. Social Cognition, 24(5), 657–701. doi:10.1521/soco.2006.24.5.657

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.