4,787
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The adolescent mind in school: theory of mind and self-concept in Canadian and Polish youth

, &
Pages 457-469 | Received 20 Feb 2013, Accepted 08 May 2013, Published online: 14 Jun 2013

References

  • Astington, J. W. (1993). The child's discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320, doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311.
  • Astington, J. W., & Pelletier, J. (1996). The language of mind: Its role in teaching and learning. In D. R. Olsen & N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development: New models of learning, teaching and schooling (pp. 593–619). Malden: Blackwell.
  • Avis, J., & Harris, P. (1991). Belief-desire reasoning among Baka children: Evidence for an universal conception of mind. Child Development, 52, 460–467.
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). The science of evil: On empathy and the origins of cruelty. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  • Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107–122.
  • Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636–644.
  • Blakemore, S. J. (2007). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 267–277.
  • Bosacki, S. (2000). Theory of mind and self-concept in preadolescents: Links with gender and language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 709–717.
  • Bosacki, S. (2008). Children's emotional lives: Sensitive shadows in the classroom. New York, NY: Peter Lang.
  • Bosacki, S., & Astington, J. (1999). Theory of mind in preadolescence: Relations between social understanding and social competence. Social Development, 8, 237–255.
  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531.
  • Bruner, J. (1996). Culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Burnett, S., Sebastian, C., Cohen Kadosh, K., & Blakemore, S. J. (2011). The social brain in adolescence: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(8), 1654–1664.
  • Callaghan, T. C., Rochat, P., Lillard, A., Claux, M. L., Odden, H., Itakura, S., … Singh, S. (2005). Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning: Evidence from 5 cultures. Psychological Science, 16, 378–384.
  • Cassidy, J. (1999). The nature of the child's ties. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory and research (pp. 3–20). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Cieciuch, J., & Rusjan, D. (2010). Polish adaptation for self-perception profile for adolescents (SPPA), S. Harter. Unpublished manuscript, Cardinal Stefan Wyszynki University.
  • Denham, S. (1998). Emotional development in young children. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive boys. Child Development, 53, 630–635.
  • Dunn, J. (1994). Changing minds and changing relationships. In M. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Origins and understanding of mind (pp. 297–310). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Dunn, J. (2008). Drama and literacy in the early years. In J. Marsh & E. Hallet (Eds.), Desirable literacies: Approaches to language and literacy in the early years. London: Paul Chapman.
  • Dunn, J., Creps, C., & Brown, J. (1996). Children's family relationships between two and five: Developmental changes and individual differences. Social Development, 5(3), 230–250.
  • Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2008). Content or connectedness? Early family talk and theory of mind in toddler and preschool years. Child Development, 79, 201–216.
  • Fox, R. (1991). Developing awareness of mind reflected in children's narrative writing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 281–298.
  • Goodnow, J., Cashmore, J., Cotton, S., & Knight, R. (1984). Mothers' developmental time tables in two cultural groups. International Journal of Psychology, 19, 193–205.
  • Harris, P. L. (2000). The work of the imagination. Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Harter, S. (1985). Manual for the self-perception profile for children (SPPC). Denver, CO: University of Denver.
  • Harter, S. (1988). Manual for the self-perception profile for adolescents (SPPA). Denver, CO: University of Denver.
  • Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Hughes, C. (2011). Social understanding and social lives: From toddlerhood through to the transition to school. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Keller, H. (2012, March). Cultural knowledge and the diversity of worldviews: What children learn through interaction. PaperSpeech presented at conference ‘Knowledge through Interaction: How Children Learn about Self, Others and Objects,’ Heidelberg, Germany.
  • Kiuru, N. (2008). The role of adolescents' peer groups in the school context. Jyväskylä: University of Jyväskylä.
  • Kovacs, A. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanism of false belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12, 48–54.
  • Larson, R. (2011). Positive development in a disorderly world. Journal of Research in Adolescence, 21, 317–334.
  • Leslie, A. M. (2000). How to acquire a ‘representational theory of mind’. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: An multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 197–223). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Liddle, B., & Nettle, D. (2006). Higher-order theory of mind and social competence in school-age children. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 4(3–4), 231–246.
  • Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardif, T., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2008). Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 523–531.
  • Lohmann, H., Tomasello, M., & Meyer, S. (2005). Linguistic communication and social understanding. In J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 245–265). Oxford: University Press.
  • Mayer, A. (2012, March). Opacity of other minds, empathy and theory of mind in Samoa. PaperSpeech presented at conference ‘Knowledge through Interaction: How Children Learn about Self, Others and Objects,’ Heidelberg, Germany.
  • Miyamoto, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2002). Culture and correspondence bias: Is the road from act to disposition rockier in Japan? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1239–1248.
  • Moriguchi, Y., Ohnishi, T., Mori, T., Matsud, H., & Komaki, G. (2007). Changes of brain activity in the neural substrates for theory of mind during childhood and adolescence. Psychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences, 61(4), 355–363.
  • Nelson, K. (2005). Language pathways into the community of minds. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 26–49). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Nelson, K. (2007). Young minds in social worlds: Experience, meaning, and memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Oberle, E., Schonert-Reichl, K., Lawlor, M., & Thomson, K. (2011). Mindfulness and inhibitory control in early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 1–24.
  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.
  • Peskin, J., & Astington, J. W. (2004). The effects of adding metacognitive language to story texts. Cognitive Development, 19(2), 253–273.
  • Peterson, C., Wellman, H., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: Advancing theory-of-mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger syndrome. Child Development, 83, 469–485.
  • Pillow, B. (2008). Development of children's understanding of cognitive activities. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169(4), 297–321.
  • Robbins, J., & Rumsey, A. (2008). Introduction: Cultural and linguistic anthropology and the opacity of other minds. Anthropological Quarterly, 81(2), 407–420.
  • Selman, R. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding. New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Sharifzadeh, V. (2004). Families with Middle Eastern roots. In E. Lynch & M. Hanson (Eds.), Developing cross-cultural competence (3rd ed., pp. 373–410). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
  • Vinden, P. G. (1999). Children's understanding of mind and emotion: A multi-culture study. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 19–48.
  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). A meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.
  • Yazdi, A. A., German, T., Defeyter, M. A., & Siegal, M. (2006). Competence and performance in belief-desire reasoning across two cultures: The truth, the whole truth and nothing but the truth about false belief? Cognition, 100, 343–368.
  • Ybrandt, H. (2008). The relation between self-concept and social functioning in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 31(1), 1–16.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.