1,947
Views
28
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
BRIEF REPORT

Higher levels of depression are associated with reduced global bias in visual processing

&
Pages 541-549 | Received 19 Mar 2013, Accepted 28 Aug 2013, Published online: 25 Sep 2013

REFERENCES

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.108.4.441
  • Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D., & Dember, W. N. (1996). Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 249–255. doi:10.1017/S1355617700001193
  • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Lee, S. C. (1997). Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 911–927. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00053-3
  • Brosch, T., Pourtois, G., & Sander, D. (2010). The perception and categorisation of emotional stimuli: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 377–400. doi:10.1080/02699930902975754
  • Caparos, S., Ahmed, L., Bremner, A. J., De Fockert, J. W., Linnell, K. J., & Davidoff, J. (2012). Exposure to an urban environment alters the local bias of a remote culture. Cognition, 122(1), 80–85. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.013
  • Caparos, S., Linnell, K. J., Bremner, A. J., de Fockert, J. W., & Davidoff, J. (2013). Do local and global perceptual biases tell us anything about local and global selective attention? Psychological Science, 24, 206–212. doi:10.1177/0956797612452569
  • Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316–336. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.3.316
  • De Fockert, J. W., Davidoff, J., Fagot, J., Parron, C., & Goldstein, J. (2007). More accurate size contrast judgments in the Ebbinghaus illusion by a remote culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 738–742. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.3.738
  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1998). Anxiety and attentional focusing: Trait, state and hemispheric influences. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 745–761. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00117-2
  • Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201. doi:10.1037/h0047707
  • Förster J. (2011). Local and global cross-modal influences between vision and hearing, tasting, smelling, or touching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 364–389. doi:10.1037/a0023175
  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 359, 1367–1378. doi:10.1098/rstb.2004.1512
  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 313–332. doi:10.1080/02699930441000238
  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global vs. local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13(1), 34–40. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00406
  • Gottfries, C. G. (2001). Late life depression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 251, 1157–1161.
  • Guinote, A. (2008). Power and affordances: When the situation has more power over powerful than powerless individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 237–252. doi:10.1037/a0012518
  • Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0
  • Holtgraves, T., & Felton, A. (2011). Hemispheric asymmetry in the processing of negative and positive words: A divided field study. Cognition & Emotion, 25, 691–699. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.493758
  • Huntsinger, J. R., Clore, G. L., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2010). Mood and Global-Local Focus: Priming a local focus reverses the link between mood and global-local processing. Emotion, 10, 722–726. doi:10.1037/a0019356
  • Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of wholistic processing and global/ local paradigm: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24–38. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.24
  • Landgraf, S., Amado, I., Purkhart, R., Ries, J., Olié, J. P., & van der Meer, E. (2011). Visuo-spatial cognition in schizophrenia: Confirmation of a preference for local information processing. Schizophrenia Research, 127, 163–170. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.027
  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual processing. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
  • Pessoa, L. (2008). Opinion: On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158.
  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES–D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306
  • Roberts, R. E. (1980). Reliability of the CES-D scale in different ethnic contexts. Psychiatry Research, 2(2), 125–134. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(80)90069-4
  • Robertson, L. C., Lamb, M. R., & Knight, R. T. (1988). Effects of lesions of temporal-parietal junction on perceptual and attentional processing in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 3757–3769.
  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime reference guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgements of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.513
  • Simonoff, E., Jones, C. R., Pickles, A., Happé, F., Baird, G., & Charman, T. (2012). Severe mood problems in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 1157–1166. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02600.x
  • Siris, S. G. (1991). Diagnosis of secondary depression in schizophrenia: Implications for DSM-IV. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17(1), 75–98. doi:10.1093/schbul/17.1.75
  • Staudinger, M. R., Fink, G. R., Mackay, C. E., & Lux, S. (2011). Gestalt perception and the decline of global precedence in older subjects. Cortex, 47, 854–862. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.08.001
  • Weissman, M. M., Sholomskas, D., Pottenger, M., Prusoff, B. A., & Locke, B. Z. (1977). Assessing depressive symptoms in five psychiatric populations: A validation study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 106, 203–214.
  • Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 3–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3
  • Yovel, I., Revelle, W., & Mineka, S. (2005). Who sees trees before forest? The obsessive compulsive style of visual attention. Psychological Science, 16(2), 123–129. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00792.x
  • Zadra, J. R., & Clore, G. L. (2011). Emotion and perception: The role of affective information. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2, 676–685. doi:10.1002/wcs.147

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.