704
Views
12
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Motivational and cognitive determinants of control during conflict processing

, &
Pages 1076-1089 | Received 10 May 2013, Accepted 25 Nov 2013, Published online: 17 Dec 2013

REFERENCES

  • Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Veltkamp, M. (2008). Goal priming and the affective-motivational route to nonconscious goal pursuit. Social Cognition, 26, 555–577. doi:10.1521/soco.2008.26.5.555
  • Adcock, R. A., Thangavel, A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Knutson, B., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2006). Reward-motivated learning: Mesolimbic activation precedes memory formation. Neuron, 50, 507–517. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.036
  • Blais, C, & Besner, D. (2006). Reverse Stroop effects with untranslated responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1345–1353. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.6.1345
  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
  • Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of physiology (pp. 602–642). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Braem, S., Verguts, T., Roggeman, C., & Notebaert, W. (2012). Reward modulates adaptations to conflict. Cognition, 125, 324–332. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.015
  • Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2007). Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, and J. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 76–106). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Bugg, J. M., & Crump, M. J. C. (2012). In support of a distinction between voluntary and stimulus-driven control: A review of the literature on proportion congruent effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 367. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367
  • Bugg, J. M., Jacoby, L. L., & Toth, J. P. (2008). Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1484–1494. doi:10.3758/MC.36.8.1484
  • Carter, C. S., Macdonald, A. M., Botvinick, M., Ross, L. L., Stenger, V. A., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. D. (2000). Parsing executive processes: Strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 1944–1948. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.4.1944
  • Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2011). Positive affect versus reward: Emotional and motivational influences on cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 279.
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences ( 2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.332
  • Della Libera, C., & Chelazzi, L. (2006). Visual selective attention and the effects of monetary rewards. Psychological Science, 17, 222–227. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01689.x
  • Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 380–390. doi:10.3758/CABN.7.4.380
  • Egner, T., Ely, S., & Grinband, J. (2010). Going, going, gone: Characterizing the time-course of congruency sequence effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 154. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00154
  • Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1784–1790. doi:10.1038/nn1594
  • Funes, M.F.Lupiáñez, J., & Humphreys, G. (2010). Sustained vs. transient control: Evidence of a behavioral dissociation. Cognition, 114, 338–347. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.007
  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.480
  • Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17. doi:10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y
  • Hübner, R., & Schlösser, J. (2010). Monetary reward increases attentional effort in the flanker task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 821–826. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.6.821
  • Huguet, P., Dumas, F., & Monteil, J. (2004). Competing for a desired reward in the Stroop task: When attentional control is unconscious but effective versus conscious but ineffective. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 153–167. doi:10.1037/h0087441
  • Jacoby, L. L., Lindsay, D. S., & Hessels, S. (2003). Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 638–644. doi:10.3758/BF03196526
  • Jimura, K., Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 8871–8876. doi:10.1073/pnas.1002007107
  • Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47
  • Kanske, P. (2012). On the influence of emotion on conflict processing. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 42. doi:10.3389/fnint.2012.00042
  • Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303, 1023–1026. doi:10.1126/science.1089910
  • Kouneiher, F., Charron, S., & Koechlin, E. (2009). Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 939–945. doi:10.1038/nn.2321
  • Krebs, R. M., Boehler, C. N., & Woldorff, M. G. (2010). The influence of reward associations on conflict processing in the Stroop task. Cognition, 117, 341–347. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.018
  • Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Stroop process dissociations: The relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 219–234. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.2.219
  • Lindström, B. R., Mattsson-Mårn, I. B., Golkar, A., & Olsson, A. (2013). In your face: Risk of punishment enhances cognitive control and error-related activity in the corrugator supercilii muscle. PLoS ONE, 8, e65692. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0065692
  • Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1998). Stroop-type interference: Congruity effects in color naming with typewritten responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 978–992. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.978
  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
  • Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects do not require executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452.
  • Müller, J., Dreisbach, G., Goschke, T., Hensch, T., Lesch, K.-P., & Brocke, B. (2007). Dopamine and cognitive control: The prospect of monetary gains influences the balance between flexibility and stability in a set-shifting paradigm. European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 3661–3668. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05949.x
  • Padmala, S., & Pessoa, L. (2011). Reward reduces conflict by enhancing attentional control and biasing visual cortical processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3419–3432. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.089
  • Rolls, E. T. (2000). Précis of the brain and emotion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 177–191; discussion 192–233. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00002429
  • Rothermund, K. (2011). Counter-regulation and control-dependency: Affective processing biases in the service of action regulation. Social Psychology, 42, 55–66. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000043
  • Savine, A. C., Beck, S. M., Edwards, B. G., Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Enhancement of cognitive control by approach and avoidance motivational states. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 338–356. doi:10.1080/02699930903381564
  • Soutschek, A., Schwarzkopf, W., Finke, K., Hennig-Fast, K., Müller, H. J., Riedel, M. … Schubert, T. (2013). Interference control in adult ADHD: No evidence for interference control deficits if response speed is controlled by delta plots. Acta Psychologica, 143, 71–78. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.02.013
  • Soutschek, A., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2013). Working memory demands modulate cognitive control in the Stroop paradigm. Psychological Research, 77, 333–347. doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0429-9
  • Soutschek, A., Taylor, P. J. C., Müller, H. J., & Schubert, T. (2013). Dissociable networks control conflict during perception and response selection: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 5647–5654. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4768-12.2013
  • Steiger, J. H. (2004). Beyond the F-test: Effect size confidence intervals and tests of close fit in the analysis of variance and contrast analysis. Psychological Methods, 9, 164–182. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.164
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651
  • Stürmer, B., Nigbur, R., Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2011). Reward and punishment effects on error processing and conflict control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 335.
  • Torres-Quesada, M., Funes, M. J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2013). Dissociating proportion congruent and conflict adaptation effects in a Simon–Stroop procedure. Acta Psychologica, 142, 203–210. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.015
  • Veling, H., & Aarts, H. (2010). Cueing task goals and earning money: Relatively high monetary rewards reduce failures to act on goals in a Stroop task. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 184–190. doi:10.1007/s11031-010-9160-2
  • Wittmann, B. C., Schott, B. H., Guderian, S., Frey, J. U., Heinze, H.-J., & Düzel, E. (2005). Reward-related fMRI activation of dopaminergic midbrain is associated with enhanced hippocampus-dependent long-term memory formation. Neuron, 45, 459–467. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.010

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.