690
Views
23
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The spider does not always win the fight for attention: Disengagement from threat is modulated by goal set

, &
Pages 1185-1196 | Received 18 Dec 2013, Accepted 21 Sep 2014, Published online: 20 Oct 2014

REFERENCES

  • Belopolsky, A. V., Devue, C., & Theeuwes, J. (2011). Angry faces hold the eyes. Visual Cognition, 19(1), 27–36. doi:10.1080/13506285.2010.536186
  • Belopolsky, A. V., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). What is top-down about contingent capture? Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 326–341. doi:10.3758/APP.72.2.326
  • Bocanegra, B. R., & Zeelenberg, R. (2009). Emotion improves and impairs early vision. Psychological Science, 20, 707–713. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02354.x
  • Brockmole, J. R., & Boot, W. R. (2009). Should I stay or should I go? Attentional disengagement from visually unique and unexpected items at fixation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 35, 808–815. doi:10.1037/a0013707
  • Brosch, T., Sander, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). That baby caught my eye. Attention capture by infant faces. Emotion, 7, 685–689. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.685
  • Dombrowe, I., Donk, M., & Olivers, C. N. L. (2011). The cost of switching attentional sets. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73, 2481–2488. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0198-3
  • Eimer, M., Kiss, M., Press, C., & Sauter, D. (2009). The roles of feature-specific task set and bottom-up salience in attentional capture: An ERP study. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1316–1328. doi:10.1037/a0015872
  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030
  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Wu, S. C. (2009). Additivity of abrupt onset effects supports nonspatial distraction, not the capture of spatial attention. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 71, 308–313. doi:10.3758/APP.71.2.308
  • Forbes, S. J., Purkis, H. M., & Lipp, O. V. (2011). Better safe than sorry: Simplistic fear-relevant stimuli capture attention. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 794–804. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.514710
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 35(1), 116–124. doi:10.3758/BF03195503
  • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R. J., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681–700. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681
  • Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355–379. doi:10.1080/02699930143000527
  • Fredrickson, M. (1983). Reliability and validity of some specific fear questionnaires. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 24, 331–334. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.1983.tb00507.x
  • Hahn, S., & Gronlund, S. D. (2007). Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 159–165. doi:10.3758/BF03194044
  • Irons, J. L., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2012). All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 758–775. doi:10.1037/a0026578
  • Isbell, L. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 1–35. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012
  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Henry Holt.
  • Kastner, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 315–341. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315
  • Klorman, R., Weerts, T. C., Hastings, J. E., Melamed, B. G., & Lang, P. J. (1974). Psychometric description of some specific-fear questionnaires. Behavior Therapy, 5, 401–409. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(74)80008-0
  • Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1183–1192. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001
  • Lachter, J., Remington, R. W., & Ruthruff, E. (2009). Space, object, and task selection. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 995–1014. doi:10.3758/APP.71.5.995
  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
  • LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155–184. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155
  • Lipp, O. V., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Attentional bias to pictures of fear relevant animals in a dot probe task. Emotion, 5, 365–369. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.365
  • Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: Attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion, 7, 192–200. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.192
  • LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: Attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychological Science, 19, 284–289. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02081.x
  • Luo, Q., Holroyd, T., Majestic, C., Cheng, X., Schechter, J., & Blair, R. J. (2010). Emotional automaticity is a matter of timing. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 5825–5829. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.BC-5668-09.2010
  • Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (1998). A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 22, 539–560. doi:10.1023/A:1018738019346
  • Mermillod, M., Droit-Volet, S., Devaux, D., Schaefer, A., & Vermeulen, N. (2010). Are coarse scales sufficient for fast detection of visual threat? Psychological Science, 21, 1429–1437. doi:10.1177/0956797610381503
  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. (2006). Time course of attentional bias for fear-relevant pictures in spider fearful individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1241–1250. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.003
  • Mogg, K., Holmes, A., Garner, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2008). Effects of threat cues on attentional shifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 46, 656–667. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.011
  • Mourão-Miranda, J., Volchan, E., Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Oliveira, L., Bramati, I., … Pessoa, L. (2003). Contributions of emotional valence and arousal to visual activation during emotional perception. NeuroImage, 20, 1950–1963. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.011
  • Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1996). A comparison of two spider fear questionnaires. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 27, 241–244. doi:10.1016/S0005-7916(96)00022-5
  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478. doi:10.1037/AXJ96-3445.130.3.466
  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.108.3.483
  • Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 773–782. doi:10.1038/nrn2920
  • Posner, M. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25. doi:10.1080/00335558008248231
  • Remington, R. W., & Folk, C. L. (2001). A dissociation between attention and selection. Psychological Science, 12, 511–515. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00394
  • Remington, R. W., Folk, C. L., & McLean, J. P. (2001). Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention? Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 298–307. doi:10.3758/BF03194470
  • Rinck, M., Reinecke, A., Ellwart, T., Heuer, K., & Becker, E. S. (2005). Speeded detection and increased distraction in fear of spiders: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 235–248. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.235
  • Schimmack, U., & Derryberry, D. (2005). Atentional interference effects of emotional pictures: Threat, negativity, or arousal? Emotion, 5(1), 55–66. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.55
  • Schreij, D., Theeuwes, J., & Olivers, C. N. L. (2010). Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings II: Additivity is no evidence for filtering. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 672–682. doi:10.3758/APP.72.3.672
  • Tamietto, M., & de Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 697–709. doi:10.1038/nrn2889
  • Theeuwes, J. (1994). Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: Selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 799–806. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.799
  • Todd, J. T., & Van Gelder, P. (1979). Implications of a transient-sustained dichotomy for the measurement of human performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 5, 625–638. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.5.4.625
  • Van Dillen, L. F., Lakens, D., & Van den Bos, K. (2011). At face value: Categorization goals modulate vigilance for angry faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 235–240. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.002
  • Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., & Van Damme, S. (2013). Competing for attentional priority: Temporary goals versus threat. Emotion, 13, 587–598. doi:10.1037/a0027204
  • Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 624–631. doi:10.1038/nn1057
  • West, G. L., Anderson, A. K., & Pratt, J. (2009). Motivationally significant stimuli show visual prior entry: Evidence for attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1032–1042. doi:10.1037/a0014493
  • Wolfe, J. M., Butcher, S. J., Lee, C., & Hyle, M. (2003). Changing your mind: On the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 483–502. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.483
  • Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.601
  • Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 3–47. doi:10.1080/02699930903205698

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.