786
Views
15
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Duration perception of emotional stimuli: Using evaluative conditioning to avoid sensory confounds

, &
Pages 1350-1367 | Received 25 Mar 2014, Accepted 15 Oct 2014, Published online: 14 Nov 2014

REFERENCES

  • Aedo-Jury, F., & Pins, D. (2010). Time compression increases with eccentricity: A magnocellular property. NeuroReport, 21(2), 84–89. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283308d57
  • Allen, J. G., & Haccoun, D. M. (1976). Sex differences in emotionality: A multidimensional approach. Human Relations, 29, 711–722. doi:10.1177/001872677602900801
  • Angrilli, A., Cherubini, P., Pavese, A., & Manfredini, S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 972–982. doi:10.3758/BF03205512
  • Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., Van den Bergh, O., & Crombez, G. (1992). The content of learning in human evaluative conditioning: Acquired valence is sensitive to US-revaluation. Learning and Motivation, 23, 200–224. doi:10.1016/0023-9690(92)90018-H
  • Baeyens, F., Field, A. P., & De Houwer, J. (2005). Associative learning of likes and dislikes: Some current controversies and possible ways forward. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 161–174. doi:10.1080/02699930441000265
  • Binkofski, F., & Block, R. A. (1996). Accelerated time experience after left frontal cortex lesion. Neurocase 2, 485–493. doi:10.1080/13554799608402424
  • Bolders, A. C., Band, G. P. H., & Stallen, P. J. (2012). Evaluative conditioning induces changes in sound valence. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 106. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00106
  • Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Sabatinelli, D., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation II: Sex differences in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 300–319. doi:10.1037//1528-3542.1.3.300
  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9
  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436. doi:10.1163/156856897X00357
  • Bruce, V., & Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 305–327. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02199.x
  • Calder, A. J., Burton, A. M., Miller, P., Young, A. W., & Akamatsu, S. (2001). A principal component analysis of facial expressions. Vision Research, 41, 1179–1208. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00002-5
  • De Houwer, J. (2007). A conceptual and theoretical analysis of evaluative conditioning. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 230–241. Retrieved from http://www.ucm.es/info/psi/docs/journal/v10n22007/art230.pdf
  • De Houwer, J. (2011). Evaluative conditioning: A review of procedure knowledge and mental process theories. In T. R. Schachtman & S. Reilly (Eds.), Associative learning and conditioning theory: Human and non-human applications (pp. 399–416). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • De Houwer, J., Baeyens, F., Vansteenwegen, D., & Eelen, P. (2000). Evaluative conditioning in the picture-picture paradigm with random assignment of conditioned stimuli to unconditioned stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 237–242. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.26.2.237
  • De Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Baeyens, F. (2001). Associative learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 853–869. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.853
  • Doi, H., & Shinohara, K. (2009). The perceived duration of emotional face is influenced by the gaze direction. Neuroscience Letters, 457, 97–100. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2009.04.004
  • Droit-Volet, S., Brunot, S., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2004). Brief report: Perception of the duration of emotional events. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 849–858. doi:10.1080/02699930341000194
  • Droit-Volet, S., Fayolle, S. L., & Gil, S. (2011). Emotion and time perception: Effects of film induced mood. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 5, 33. doi:10.3389/fnint.2011.00033
  • Droit-Volet, S., & Gil, S. (2009). The time-emotion paradox. Philosophical Transactions from the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 364, 1943–1953. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0013
  • Droit-Volet, S., & Meck, W. H. (2007). How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 504–513. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.008
  • Droit-Volet, S., Mermillod, M., Cocenas-Silva, R., & Gil, S. (2010). The effect of expectancy of a threatening event on time perception in human adults. Emotion, 10, 908–914. doi:0.1037/a0020258
  • Eagleman, D. M. (2008). Human time perception and its illusions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 131–136. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.008
  • Eimer, M. (2011). The face-sensitive N170 component of the event-related brain potential. In J. A. Calder, G. Rhodes, M. Johnson, & J. Haxby (Eds.), Oxford handbook of face perception (pp. 329–344).
  • Effron, D. A., Niedenthal, P. M., Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2006). Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion, 6(1), 1–9. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.1
  • Gast, A., Gawronski, B., & De Houwer, J. (2012). Evaluative conditioning: Recent developments and future directions. Learning and Motivation, 43(3), 79–88. doi:10.1016/j.lmot.2012.06.004
  • Gauthier, I., & Logothetis, N. K. (2000). Is face recognition not so unique after all? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(1–3), 125–142. doi:10.1080/026432900380535
  • Gawronski, B., & Mitchell, D. G. V. (2014). Simultaneous conditioning of valence and arousal. Cognition & Emotion, 28, 577–595. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.843506
  • Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2011). “Time flies in the presence of angry faces” … depending on the temporal task used! Acta Psychologica, 136, 354–362. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.12.010
  • Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). Emotional time distortions: The fundamental role of arousal. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 847–862. doi:10.1080/02699931.2011.625401
  • Gil, S., Niedenthal, P. M., & Droit-Volet, S. (2007). Anger and time perception in children. Emotion, 7(1), 219–225. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.219
  • Grommet, E. K., Droit-Volet, S., Gil, S., Hemmes, N. S., Baker, A. H., & Brown, B. L. (2011). Time estimation of fear cues in human observers. Behavioural Processes, 86(1), 88–93. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.10.003
  • Grondin, S. (2008). Methods of studying psychological time. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 51–75). Bingley: Emerald.
  • Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 561–582. doi:10.3758/APP.72.3.561
  • Hammerl, M., & Grabitz, H.-J. (2000). Affective-evaluative learning in humans: A form of associative learning or only an artifact? Learning and Motivation, 31, 345–363. doi:10.1006/lmot.2000.1059
  • Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper & Row.
  • Hicks, R. E., Miller, G. W., & Kinsbourne, M. (1976). Prospective and retrospective judgments of time as a function of amount of information processed. The American Journal of Psychology, 89, 719–730. doi:10.2307/1421469
  • Hillman, C. H., Rosengren, K. S., & Smith, D. P. (2004). Emotion and motivated behavior: Postural adjustments to affective picture viewing. Biological Psychology, 66(1), 51–62. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2003.07.005
  • Hofmann, W., De Houwer, J., Perugini, M., Baeyens, F., & Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 390–421. doi:10.1037/a0018916
  • Hornik, J. (1992). Time estimation and orientation mediated by transient mood. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 21, 209–227. doi:10.1016/1053-5357(92)90010-5
  • Jones, C. R., Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2009). Implicit misattribution as a mechanism underlying evaluative conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 933–948. doi:10.1037/a0014747
  • Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697
  • Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–4311.
  • Karama, S., Lecours, A. R., Leroux, J.-M., Bourgouin, P., Beaudoin, G., Joubert, S., & Beauregard, M. (2002). Areas of brain activation in males and females during viewing of erotic film excerpts. Human Brain Mapping, 16(1), 1–13. doi:10.1002/hbm.10014
  • Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (1998). Emotion, social function, and psychopathology. Review of General Psychology, 2, 320–342. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.320
  • Kliegl, K. M., & Huckauf, A. (2014). Perceived duration decreases with increasing eccentricity. Acta Psychologica, 150, 136–145. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.007
  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical Report A-8). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.
  • Levey, A. B., & Martin, I. (1975). Classical conditioning of human ‘evaluative’ responses. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 13, 221–226. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(75)90026-1
  • Long, G. M., & Beaton, R. J. (1981). The effects of stimulus numerosity, retinal location, and rod contrast on perceived duration of brief visual stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 389–394. doi:10.3758/BF03207349
  • Manstead, A. S. R., Wagner, H. L., & MacDonald, C. J. (1983). A contrast effect in judgments of own emotional state. Motivation and Emotion, 7, 279–290. doi:10.1007/BF00991678
  • Martijn, C., Sheeran, P., Wesseldijk, L. W., Merrick, H., Webb, T. L., Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2013). Evaluative conditioning makes slim models less desirable as standards for comparison and increases body satisfaction. Health Psychology, 32, 433–438. doi:10.1037/a0028592
  • Martin, I., & Levey, A. B. (1978). Evaluative conditioning. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1(2), 57–101. doi:10.1016/0146-6402(78)90013-9
  • Matthews, W. J. (2013). How does sequence structure affect the judgment of time? Exploring a weighted sum of segments model. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 259–282. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.01.001
  • Matthews, W. J. (2011). Stimulus repetition and the perception of time: The effects of prior exposure on temporal discrimination, judgment, and production. PloS One, 6(5), e19815. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019815
  • McKone, E., Kanwisher, N., & Duchaine, B. C. (2007). Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 8–15. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.002
  • Mella, N., Conty, L., & Pouthas, V. (2011). The role of physiological arousal in time perception: Psychophysiological evidence from an emotion regulation paradigm. Brain and Cognition, 75, 182–187. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.012
  • Noulhiane, M., Mella, N., Samson, S., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2007). How emotional auditory stimuli modulate time perception. Emotion, 7, 697–704. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.697
  • Ono, F., & Kawahara, J.-I. (2007). The subjective size of visual stimuli affects the perceived duration of their presentation. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 952–957. doi:10.3758/BF03193932
  • Pham, M. T. (2007). Emotion and rationality: A critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review of General Psychology, 11, 155–178. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.155
  • Povel, D.-J., & Essens, J. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception, 2, 411–440. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/40285311
  • Roussel, M.-E., Grondin, S., & Killeen, P. (2009). Spatial effects on temporal categorisation. Perception, 38, 748–762. doi:10.1068/p6165
  • Russell, J. A. (1979). Affective space is bipolar. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 345–356. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.3.345
  • Russell, J. A., Lewicka, M., & Niit, T. (1989). A cross-cultural study of a circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 848–856. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.848
  • Russell, J. A., & Mehrabian, A. (1977). Evidence for a three-factor theory of emotions. Journal of Research in Personality, 11, 273–294. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(77)90037-X
  • Schiffman, H. R., & Bobko, D. J. (1974). Effects of stimulus complexity on the perception of brief temporal intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 156–159. doi:10.1037/h0036794
  • Schirmer A. (2011). How emotions change time. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 5, 58. doi:10.3389/fnint.2011.00058
  • Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjective well-being: Judgmental processes and their methodological implications. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 61–84). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
  • Smith, S. D., McIver, T. A., Di Nella, M. S. J., & Crease, M. L. (2011). The effects of valence and arousal on the emotional modulation of time perception: Evidence for multiple stages of processing. Emotion, 11, 1305–1313. doi:10.1037/a0026145
  • Thomas, E. A. C., & Cantor, N. E. (1975). On the duality of simultaneous time and size perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 18(1), 44–48. doi:10.3758/BF03199365
  • Tipples, J. (2008). Negative emotionality influences the effects of emotion on time perception. Emotion, 8(1), 127–131. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.1.127
  • Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 77(13), 1–31. doi:10.1037/h0093864
  • Treisman, M., & Brogan, D. (1992). Time perception and the internal clock: Effects of visual flicker on the temporal oscillator. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4(1), 41–70. doi:10.1080/09541449208406242
  • Walther, E., & Nagengast, B. (2006). Evaluative conditioning and the awareness issue: Assessing contingency awareness with the Four-Picture Recognition Test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 454–459. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.32.4.454
  • Watts, F. N., & Sharrock, R. (1984). Fear and time estimation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59, 597–598. doi:10.2466/pms.1984.59.2.597
  • Wilmer, J. B., Germine, L., Chabris, C. F., Chatterjee, G., Williams, M., Loken, E., Nakayama, K., & Duchaine, B. (2010). Human face recognition ability is specific and highly heritable. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 5238–5241. doi:10.1073/pnas.0913053107
  • Wittmann, M. (2009). The inner experience of time. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1955–1967. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0003
  • Xuan, B., Zhang, D., He, S., & Chen, X. (2007). Larger stimuli are judged to last longer. Journal of Vision, 7(10), 2. doi:10.1167/7.10.2

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.