318
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Fast and unintentional evaluation of emotional sounds: evidence from brief segment ratings and the affective Simon task

&
Pages 312-324 | Received 28 Oct 2014, Accepted 16 Oct 2015, Published online: 20 Nov 2015

References

  • Bargh, J. A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: Toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. American Journal of Psychology, 105, 181–199. doi:10.2307/1423027
  • Bendixen, A., SanMiguel, I., & Schröger, E. (2012). Early electrophysiological indicators for predictive processing in audition: A review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83, 120–131. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.08.003
  • Bermeitinger, C., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2011). How to switch on and switch off semantic priming effects for natural and artifactual categories: Activation processes in category memory depend on focusing specific feature dimensions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 579–585. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0067-z
  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). The International Affective Digitized Sounds: Affective ratings of sounds and instruction manual (2nd ed., IADS-2, Tech. Rep. B-3). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.
  • Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis. The perceptual organization of sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Bröckelmann, A.-K., Steinberg, C., Dobel, C., Elling, L., Zwanzger, P., Pantev, C., & Junghöfer, M. (2013). Affect-specific modulation of the N1m to shock-conditioned tones: Magnetoencephalographic correlates. European Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 303–315. doi:10.1111/ejn.12043
  • Bröckelmann, A.-K., Steinberg, C., Elling, L., Zwanzger, P., Pantev, C., & Junghofer, M. (2011). Emotion-associated tones attract enhanced attention at early auditory processing: Magnetoencephalographic correlates. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 7801–7810. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.6236-10.2011
  • Carroll, K., & Young, A. W. (2005). Priming of emotion recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 58, 1173–1197. doi:10.1080/02724980443000539
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Czigler, I., Cox, T. J., Gyimesi, K., & Horváth, J. (2007). Event-related potential study to aversive auditory stimuli. Neuroscience Letters, 420, 251–256. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2007.05.007
  • De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., Baeyens, F., & Hermans, D. (2001). On the generality of the affective Simon effect. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 189–206. doi:10.1080/02699930125883
  • De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1998). An affective variant of the Simon paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 45–62. doi:10.1080/026999398379772
  • Degner, J. (2011). Affective priming with auditory speech stimuli. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1710–1735. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.532625
  • Dolan, R. J., Morris, J. S., & de Gelder, B. (2001). Crossmodal binding of fear in voice and face. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 10006–10010. doi:10.1073/pnas.171288598
  • Everaert, T., Spruyt, A., & De Houwer, J. (2013). On the malleability of automatic attentional biases: Effects of feature-specific attention allocation. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 385–400. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.712949
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.
  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030
  • Folyi, T., Liesefeld, H. R., & Wentura, D. (2015). Attentional enhancement for positive and negative tones at an early stage of auditory processing. Manuscript submitted for publication.
  • Goerlich, K. S., Witteman, J., Schiller, N. O., Van Heuven, V. J., Aleman, A., & Martens, S. (2012). The nature of affective priming in music and speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1725–1741. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00213
  • Grandjean, D., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., Schwartz, S., Seghier, M. L., Scherer, K. R., & Vuilleumier, P. (2005). The voices of wrath: Brain responses to angry prosody in meaningless speech. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 145–146. doi:10.1038/nn1392
  • Griffiths, T. D., & Warren, J. D. (2004). What is an auditory object? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 887–892. doi:10.1038/nrn1538
  • Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2001). A time course analysis of the affective priming effect. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 143–165. doi:10.1080/02699930125768
  • Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: Task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 464–489. doi:10.1037/a0019561
  • King, A. J., & Nelken, I. (2009). Unraveling the principles of auditory cortical processing: Can we learn from the visual system? Nature Neuroscience, 12, 698–701. doi:10.1038/nn.2308
  • Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: Findings and theories. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 7–50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Klauer, K. C., Roßnagel, C., & Musch, J. (1997). List context effects in evaluative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 246–255. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.1.246
  • Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., von Cramon, D. Y., Müller, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Investigating emotion with music: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 239–250. doi:10.1002/hbm.20180
  • Marin, M. M., Gingras, B., & Bhattacharya, J. (2012). Crossmodal transfer of arousal, but not pleasantness, from the musical to the visual domain. Emotion, 12, 618–631. doi:10.1037/a0025020
  • Mitchell, R. L., Elliott, R., Barry, M., Cruttenden, A., & Woodruff, P. W. (2003). The neural response to emotional prosody, as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1410–1421. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00017-4
  • Moors, A. (2015). Automaticity: Componential, causal, and mechanistic explanations. Annual Review of Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033550
  • Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2001). Automatic appraisal of motivational valence: Motivational affective priming and Simon effects. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 749–766. doi:10.1080/02699930143000293
  • Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297–326. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297
  • Näätänen, R., Paavilainen, P., Rinne, T., & Alho, K. (2007). The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2544–2590. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.026
  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466
  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483
  • Pourtois, G., de Gelder, B., Bol, A., & Crommelinck, M. (2005). Perception of facial expressions and voices and of their combination in the human brain. Cortex, 41, 49–59. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70177-1
  • Rosenblum, L. D., Carello, C., & Pastore, R. E. (1987). Relative effectiveness of three stimulus variables for locating a moving sound source. Perception, 16, 175–186.
  • Sander, K., Frome, Y., & Scheich, H. (2007). FMRI activations of amygdala, cingulate cortex, and auditory cortex by infant laughing and crying. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 1007–1022. doi:10.1002/hbm.20333
  • Sander, K., & Scheich, H. (2001). Auditory perception of laughing and crying activates human amygdala regardless of attentional state. Cognitive Brain Research, 12, 181–198. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00045-3
  • Sauter, D. A., & Eimer, M. (2010). Rapid detection of emotion from human vocalizations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 474–481. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21215
  • Scherer, L. D., & Larsen, R. J. (2011). Cross-modal evaluative priming: Emotional sounds influence the processing of emotion words. Emotion, 11, 203–208. doi:10.1037/a0022588
  • Schirmer, A., Kotz, S. A., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Sex differentiates the role of emotional prosody during word processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 228–233. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00108-8
  • Scott, S. K., Sauter, D., & McGettigan, C. (2009). Brain mechanisms for processing perceived emotional vocalizations in humans. In S. Brudzynski (Ed.), Handbook of mammalian vocalizations: An integrative neuroscience approach (pp. 187–198). Oxford: Academic Press.
  • Shinkareva, S. V., Wang, J., Kim, J., Facciani, M. J., Baucom, L. B., & Wedell, D. H. (2014). Representations of modality-specific affective processing for visual and auditory stimuli derived from functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 3558–3568. doi:10.1002/hbm.22421
  • Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420–428. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420
  • Sollberger, B., Rebe, R., & Eckstein, D. (2003). Musical chords as affective priming context in a word-evaluation task. Music Perception, 20, 263–282. doi:10.1525/mp.2003.20.3.263
  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Everaert, T., & Hermans, D. (2012). Unconscious semantic activation depends on feature-specific attention allocation. Cognition, 122, 91–95. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.017
  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Hermans, D. (2009). Modulation of automatic semantic priming by feature-specific attention allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 37–54. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.03.004
  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., & Eelen, P. (2007). Affective priming of nonaffective semantic categorization responses. Experimental Psychology, 54, 44–53. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.44
  • Steinbeis, N., & Koelsch, S. (2011). Affective priming effects of musical sounds on the processing of word meaning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 604–621. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21383
  • Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Vermeulen, N., Corneille, O., & Luminet, O. (2007). A mood moderation of the extrinsic affective Simon task. European Journal of Personality, 21, 359–369. doi:10.1002/per.621
  • Voß, A., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2003). Estimating the valence of single stimuli: A new variant of the affective Simon task. Experimental Psychology, 50, 86–96. doi:10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.86
  • Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: Neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 585–594. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011
  • Weninger, F., Eyben, F., Schuller, B. W., Mortillaro, M., & Scherer, K. R. (2013). On the acoustics of emotion in audio: What speech, music, and sound have in common. Frontiers in Psychology, Emotion Science, 4, 1–12. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00292
  • Wentura, D., & Degner, J. (2010). A practical guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 95–116). New York: Guilford.
  • Wentura, D., Müller, P., & Rothermund, K. (2014). Attentional capture by evaluative stimuli: Gain- and loss-connoting colors boost the additional-singleton effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 701–707. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0531-z
  • Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (2003). The “meddling-in” of affective information: A general model of automatic evaluation effects. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 51–86). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 3–47. doi:10.1080/02699930903205698

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.