496
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The go/no-go priming task: automatic evaluation and categorisation beyond response interference

&
Pages 892-911 | Received 05 Oct 2015, Accepted 12 Apr 2016, Published online: 05 May 2016

References

  • Borkenau, P., & Mauer, S. (2007). Well-being and the accessibility of pleasant and unpleasant concepts. European Journal of Personality, 21, 169–189. doi:10.1002/per.613
  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Cohen Kadosh, R., Gevers, W., & Notebaert, W. (2011). Sequential analysis of the numerical Stroop effect reveals response suppression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 37, 1243–1249.
  • Degner, J. (2009). On the (un)controllability of affective priming: Strategic manipulation is feasible but can possibly be prevented. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 327–354. doi: 10.1080/02699930801993924
  • Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Clec’H, L. G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., … Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597–600. doi: 10.1038/26967
  • De Houwer, J. (2003). A structural analysis of indirect measures of attitudes. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 219–244). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2002). Affective priming of semantic categorization responses. Cognition & Emotion, 16, 643–666. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000419
  • Draine, S. C., & Greenwald, A. G. (1998). Replicable unconscious semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 286–303. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.127.3.286
  • Duthoo, W., Abrahamse, E. L., Braem, S., Boehler, C. N., & Notebaert, W. (2014). The heterogeneous world of congruency sequence effects: An update. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 ( Article 1001). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01001
  • Egner, T., Ely, S., & Grinband, J. (2010). Going, going, gone: Characterizing the time-course of congruency sequence effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00154
  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
  • Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229
  • Frings, C., Schneider, K. K., & Fox, E. (2015). The negative priming paradigm – An update and implications for selective attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1577–1597. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0841-4
  • Frings, C., & Wentura, D. (2008). Trial-by-trial effects in the affective priming paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 128, 318–323. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.03.004
  • Frings, C., Wentura, D., & Holtz, M. (2007). Dysphorics cannot ignore unpleasant information. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 1525–1534. doi: 10.1080/02699930601054042
  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.480
  • Herring, D. R., White, K. R., Jabeen, L. N., Hinojos, M., Terrazas, G., Reyes, S. M., … Crites, S. L. (2013). On the automatic activation of attitudes: A quarter century of evaluative priming research. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1062–1089. doi: 10.1037/a0031309
  • Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17. doi: 10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y
  • Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 125–147. doi: 10.1080/02699930244000480
  • Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching–A review. Psychological Bulletin 136, 849–874. doi:10.1037/a0019842
  • Klauer, K. C., Eder, A. B., Greenwald, A. G., & Abrams, R. L. (2007). Priming of semantic classifications by novel subliminal prime words. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 63–83. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.12.002
  • Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: Findings and theories. In J. Musch, & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 7–49). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Klauer, K. C., Roßnagel, C., & Musch, J. (1997). List context effects in evaluative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 246–255.
  • Klinger, M. R., Burton, P. C., & Pitts, G. S. (2000). Mechanisms of unconscious priming: I. Response competition not spreading activation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 441–455.
  • Kopp, B., Rist, F., & Mattler, U. (1996). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology, 33(3), 282–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb00425.x
  • Kunde, W., Augst, S., & Kleinsorge, T. (2012). Adaptation to (non) valent task disturbance. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 644–660. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0116-8
  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville: University of Florida.
  • Logan, G. D. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 523–553. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90019-5
  • Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (1990). Designing experiments and analyzing data. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.
  • Musch, J., & Klauer, K. C. (Eds.). (2003). The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Oswald, W. D., & Roth, E. (1987). Der Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test (ZVT). Ein sprachfreier Intelligenz-Test zur Messung der “kognitiven Leistungsgeschwindigkeit”. Handanweisung (2, überarbeitete und erweiterte Auflage). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
  • de Paula Couto, M. C. P., & Wentura, D. (2012). Automatically activated facets of ageism: Masked evaluative priming allows for a differentiation of age-related prejudice. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 852–863. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1912
  • Perea, M., Rosa, E., & Gomez, C. (2002). Is the go/no-go lexical decision task an alternative to the yes/no lexical decision task? Memory & Cognition, 30, 34–45. doi: 10.3758/BF03195263
  • Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [E-Prime 2.0]. (2012). Retrieved from http://www.pstnet.com
  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207
  • Schmidt, J. R. (2013). Questioning conflict adaptation: Proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 615–630. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0373-0
  • Schmidt, J. R., & De Houwer, J. (2011). Now you see it, now you don't: Controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions eliminates the Gratton effect. Acta Psychologica, 138, 176–186. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.06.002
  • Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor, & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  • Spruyt, A., Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2002). On the nature of the affective priming effect: Affective priming of naming responses. Social Cognition, 20, 227–256. doi: 10.1521/soco.20.3.227.21106
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651
  • Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Klauer, K. C. (2013). On the controllability of evaluative-priming effects: Some limits that are none. Cognition & Emotion, 27, 632–657. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.732041
  • Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590. doi: 10.1080/14640748508400920
  • Wentura, D. (1999). Activation and inhibition of affective information: Evidence for negative priming in the evaluation task. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 65–91. doi: 10.1080/026999399379375
  • Wentura, D., & Degner, J. (2010). A practical guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 95–116). New York: Guilford.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.