498
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular Articles

The contextual malleability of approach-avoidance training effects: approaching or avoiding fear conditioned stimuli modulates effects of approach-avoidance training

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 341-349 | Received 25 Jul 2016, Accepted 12 Mar 2017, Published online: 27 Mar 2017

References

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 1.1-7. R. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
  • Becker, D., Jostmann, N. B., Wiers, R. W., & Holland, R. W. (2015). Approach avoidance training in the eating domain: Testing the effectiveness across three single session studies. Appetite, 85 (April 2016), 58–65. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.017
  • Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(1), 5–17. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.5
  • Centerbar, D. B., & Clore, G. L. (2006). Do approach-avoidance actions create attitudes? Psychological Science, 17(1), 22–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01660.x
  • Eder, A. B., & Rothermund, K. (2008). When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 262–281. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.262
  • Eder, A. B., Rothermund, K., De Houwer, J., & Boraud, T. (2013). Affective compatibility between stimuli and response goals: A primer for a new implicit measure of attitudes. PloS One, 8(11), e79210. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079210
  • Gast, A., & Rothermund, K. (2011a). I like it because I said that I like it: Evaluative conditioning effects can be based on stimulus-response learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 37(4), 466–476. doi: 10.1037/a0023077
  • Gast, A., & Rothermund, K. (2011b). What you see is what will change: Evaluative conditioning effects depend on a focus on valence. Cognition & Emotion, 25(1), 89–110. doi: 10.1080/02699931003696380
  • Hsee, C. K., Tu, Y., Lu, Z. Y., & Ruan, B. (2014). Approach aversion: Negative hedonic reactions toward approaching stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(5), 699–712. doi: 10.1037/a0036332
  • Hughes, S., De Houwer, J., & Perugini, M. (2016). Expanding the boundaries of evaluative learning research: How intersecting regularities shape our likes and dislikes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(6), 731–754. doi: 10.1037/xge0000100
  • Jones, C. R., Vilensky, M. R., Vasey, M. W., & Fazio, R. H. (2013). Approach behavior can mitigate predominately univalent negative attitudes: Evidence regarding insects and spiders. Emotion, 13(5), 989–996. doi: 10.1037/a0033164
  • Kawakami, K., Phills, C. E., Steele, J. R., & Dovidio, J. F. (2007). (Close) distance makes the heart grow fonder: Improving implicit racial attitudes and interracial interactions through approach behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 957–971. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.957
  • Krieglmeyer, R., De Houwer, J., & Deutsch, R. (2011). How farsighted are behavioral tendencies of approach and avoidance? The effect of stimulus valence on immediate vs. ultimate distance change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 622–627. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.12.021
  • Krieglmeyer, R., De Houwer, J., & Deutsch, R. (2013). On the nature of automatically triggered approach–avoidance behavior. Emotion Review, 5(3), 280–284. doi: 10.1177/1754073913477501
  • Krieglmeyer, R., Deutsch, R., De Houwer, J., & De Raedt, R. (2010). Being moved: Valence activates approach-avoidance behavior independently of evaluation and approach-avoidance intentions. Psychological Science, 21(4), 607–613. doi: 10.1177/0956797610365131
  • Krypotos, A.-M., Arnaudova, I., Effting, M., Kindt, M., Beckers, T., & Dymond, S. (2015). Effects of approach-avoidance training on the extinction and return of fear responses. PLOS ONE, 10(7), e0131581. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131581
  • Laham, S. M., Kashima, Y., Dix, J., Wheeler, M., & Levis, B. (2014). Elaborated contextual framing is necessary for action-based attitude acquisition. Cognition and Emotion, 28(6), 1119–1126. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.867833
  • Maison, D., Greenwald, A. G., & Bruin, R. H. (2004). Predictive validity of the implicit association test in studies of brands, consumer attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 405–415. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1404_9
  • McConnell, A. R., & Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the implicit association test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37(5), 435–442. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2000.1470
  • Mertens, G., & De Houwer, J. (2016). Potentiation of the startle reflex is in line with contingency reversal instructions rather than the conditioning history. Biological Psychology, 113, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.11.014
  • Neumann, R., Förster, J., & Strack, F. (2003). Motor compatibility: The bidirectional link between behavior and evaluation. In J. Musch & K. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 371–391). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Shanks, D. R., & Berry, C. J. (2012). Are there multiple memory systems? Tests of models of implicit and explicit memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(8), 1449–1474. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.691887
  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Hermans, D. (2009). Modulation of automatic semantic priming by feature-specific attention allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(1), 37–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.03.004
  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Hermans, D., & Eelen, P. (2007). Affective priming of nonaffective semantic categorization responses. Experimental Psychology, 54(1), 44–53. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.44
  • Tibboel, H., De Houwer, J., Spruyt, A., Brevers, D., Roy, E., & Noël, X. (2015). Heavy social drinkers score higher on implicit wanting and liking for alcohol than alcohol-dependent patients and light social drinkers. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 48, 185–191. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.04.003
  • Van Dessel, P., De Houwer, J., & Gast, A. (2016). Approach-avoidance training effects are moderated by awareness of stimulus-action contingencies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(1), 81–93. doi: 10.1177/0146167215615335
  • Van Dessel, P., De Houwer, J., Roets, A., & Gast, A. (2016). Failures to change stimulus evaluations by means of subliminal approach and avoidance training. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(1), e1–e15. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000039
  • van Uijen, S., van den Hout, M., & Engelhard, I. (2015). Active approach does not add to the effects of in vivo exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 6(1), 112–125. doi: 10.5127/jep.042014
  • Vandenbosch, K., & De Houwer, J. (2011). Failures to induce implicit evaluations by means of approach-avoid training. Cognition & Emotion, 25(7), 1311–1330; discussion 1331–8. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.596819
  • Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychological Science, 22(4), 490–497. doi: 10.1177/0956797611400615
  • Wittenbrink, B. (2007). Measuring attitudes through priming. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes: Progress and controversies (pp. 17–58). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Woud, M. L., Becker, E. S., Lange, W.-G., & Rinck, M. (2013). Effects of approach-avoidance training on implicit and explicit evaluations of neutral, angry, and smiling face stimuli. Psychological Reports, 113(1), 199–216. doi:10.2466/21.07.PR0.113×10z1 doi: 10.2466/21.07.PR0.113x10z1

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.