1,128
Views
21
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Brief Articles

Arousal (but not valence) amplifies the impact of salience

&
Pages 616-622 | Received 05 Jan 2017, Accepted 05 May 2017, Published online: 01 Jun 2017

References

  • Becker, M. W. (2009). Panic search: Fear produces efficient visual search for nonthreatening objects. Psychological Science, 20(4), 435–437. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02303.x
  • Bocanegra, B. R., & Zeelenberg, R. (2009). Dissociating emotion-induced blindness and hypervision. Emotion, 9(6), 865–873. doi: 10.1037/a0017749
  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). The International Affective Digitized Sounds (2nd Edition; IADS-2): Affective ratings of sounds and instruction manual. Technical report B-3. Gainesville: University of Florida.
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Fecteau, J. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2006). Salience, relevance, and firing: A priority map for target selection. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 382–390. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.011
  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 313–332. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000238
  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13(1), 34–40. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00406
  • Han, H., Gabriel, K. P., & Kohl, H. W. (2015). Evaluations of validity and reliability of a transtheoretical model for sedentary behavior among college students. American Journal of Health Behavior, 39(5), 601–609. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.39.5.2
  • Huntsinger, J. R. (2013). Does emotion directly tune the scope of attention? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(4), 265–270. doi: 10.1177/0963721413480364
  • Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40(10–12), 1489–1506. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00163-7
  • Lee, T. H., Itti, L., & Mather, M. (2012). Evidence for arousal-biased competition in perceptual learning. Frontiers in Emotion Science, 3, 241.
  • Lee, T. H., Sakaki, M., Cheng, R., Velasco, R., & Mather, M. (2014). Emotional arousal amplifies the effects of biased competition in the brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(12), 2067–2077. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu015
  • Mather, M., Clewett, D., Sakaki, M., & Harley, C. W. (2016). Norepinephrine ignites local hot spots of neuronal excitation: How arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, e200. https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences/article/divclasstitlenorepinephrine-ignites-local-hotspots-of-neuronal-excitation-how-arousal-amplifies-selectivity-in-perception-andmemorydiv/A1750B4C91812D0CC7F6D42872DC05AD doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15000667
  • Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 114–133. doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234
  • Olatunji, B. O., Ciesielski, B. G., Armstrong, T., & Zald, D. H. (2011). Emotional expressions and visual search efficiency: Specificity and effects of anxiety symptoms. Emotion, 11(5), 1073–1079. doi: 10.1037/a0021785
  • Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2004). Memory for emotional events. In D. Reisberg, & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and emotion (pp. 3–41). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(1), 383–388. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605198104
  • Sakaki, M., Fryer, K., & Mather, M. (2014). Emotion strengthens high-priority memory traces but weakens low-priority memory traces. Psychological Science, 25, 387–395. doi: 10.1177/0956797613504784
  • Sutherland, M. R., & Mather, M. (2012). Negative arousal amplifies the effects of saliency in short-term memory. Emotion, 12, 1367–1372. doi: 10.1037/a0027860
  • Sutherland, M. R., & Mather, M. (2015). Negative arousal increases the effects of stimulus salience in older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 41(3), 259–271. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2015.1021644
  • Wang, L., Kennedy, B. L., & Most, S. B. (2012). When emotion blinds: A spatiotemporal competition account of emotion-induced blindness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3.
  • Yegiyan, N. S., & Lang, A. (2010). Processing central and peripheral detail: How content arousal and emotional tone influence encoding. Media Psychology, 13(1), 77–99. doi: 10.1080/15213260903563014

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.