592
Views
12
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Identification and location tasks rely on different mental processes: a diffusion model account of validity effects in spatial cueing paradigms with emotional stimuli

, &
Pages 231-244 | Received 09 Jun 2017, Accepted 16 Feb 2018, Published online: 22 Feb 2018

References

  • Amir, N., Elias, J., Klumpp, H., & Przeworski, A. (2003). Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: Facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1325–1335. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00039-1
  • Chica, A. B., Martin-Arévalo, E., Botta, F., & Lupiáñez, J. (2014). The spatial orienting paradigm: How to design and interpret spatial attention experiments. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 40, 35–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.002
  • Clarke, P. J., MacLeod, C., & Guastella, A. J. (2013). Assessing the role of spatial engagement and disengagement of attention in anxiety-linked attentional bias: A critique of current paradigms and suggestions for future research directions. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 26, 1–19. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2011.638054
  • Compton, R. J. (2000). Ability to disengage attention predicts negative affect. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 401–415. doi: 10.1080/026999300378897
  • Cooper, R. M., Rowe, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Ludwig, C. (2009). No reliable effects of emotional facial expression, adult attachment orientation, or anxiety on the allocation of visual attention in the spatial cueing paradigm. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 643–652. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.03.005
  • Crusius, J., & Lange, J. (2014). What catches the envious eye? Attentional biases within malicious and benign envy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.05.007
  • Ellenbogen, M. A., Schwartzman, A. E., Stewart, J., & Walker, C. D. (2002). Stress and selective attention: The interplay of mood, cortisol levels, and emotional information processing. Psychophysiology, 39, 723–732. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3960723
  • Everaert, J., Tierens, M., Uzieblo, K., & Koster, E. H. (2013). The indirect effect of attention bias on memory via interpretation bias: Evidence for the combined cognitive bias hypothesis in subclinical depression. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 1450–1459. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.787972
  • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681–700. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681
  • Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355–379. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000527
  • Harrison, N. R., & Woodhouse, R. (2016). Modulation of auditory spatial attention by visual emotional cues: Differential effects of attentional engagement and disengagement for pleasant and unpleasant cues. Cognitive Processing, 17, 205–211. doi: 10.1007/s10339-016-0749-6
  • Koster, E., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2005). Signals for threat modulate attentional capture and holding: Fear-conditioning and extinction during the exogenous cueing task. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 771–780. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000418
  • Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., Van Damme, S., & Wiersema, J. R. (2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: Facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1757–1771. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.011
  • Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2010). Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: Exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 219–225. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.004
  • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15
  • Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., Rouby, D. A., & Miller, S. L. (2007). Can’t take my eyes off you: Attentional adhesion to mates and rivals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 389–401. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.389
  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Some methodological issues in assessing attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: A replication study using a modified version of the probe detection task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 595–604. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00158-2
  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483
  • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Puls, S., & Rothermund, K. (2017). Attending to emotional expressions: No evidence for automatic capture in the dot-probe task. Cognition and Emotion. Advanced Online Publication. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1314932
  • Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (2008). The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Computation, 20, 873–922. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420
  • Schmukle, S. C. (2005). Unreliability of the dot probe task. European Journal of Personality, 19, 595–605. doi: 10.1002/per.554
  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Pearson.
  • Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., Koster, E. H., Van Damme, S., & Crombez, G. (2008). Allocation of spatial attention to emotional stimuli depends upon arousal and not valence. Emotion, 8, 880–885. doi: 10.1037/a0013981
  • Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., Moors, A., Van Damme, S., & Crombez, G. (2010). The automatic orienting of attention to goal-relevant stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 134, 61–69. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.12.006
  • Voss, A., Nagler, M., & Lerche, V. (2013). Diffusion models in experimental psychology. Experimental Psychology, 60, 385–402. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000218
  • Voss, A., Rothermund, K., & Brandtstädter, J. (2008). Interpreting ambiguous stimuli: Separating perceptual and judgmental biases. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1048–1056. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.009
  • Voss, A., Rothermund, K., & Voss, J. (2004). Interpreting the parameters of the diffusion model: An empirical validation. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1206–1220. doi: 10.3758/BF03196893
  • Voss, A., & Voss, J. (2007). Fast-dm: A free program for efficient diffusion model analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 767–775. doi: 10.3758/BF03192967
  • Wirth, B. E., & Wentura, D. (2018). Attentional bias to threat in the general population is contingent on target competition, not on attentional control settings. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1307864
  • Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 3–47. doi: 10.1080/02699930903205698

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.