4,004
Views
14
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The impact of past behaviour normality on regret: replication and extension of three experiments of the exceptionality effect

& ORCID Icon
Pages 901-914 | Received 25 Sep 2017, Accepted 21 Jul 2018, Published online: 10 Aug 2018

References

  • Bear, A., & Knobe, J. (2017). Normality: Part descriptive, part prescriptive. Cognition, 167, 25–37. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.024
  • Briazu, R. A., Walsh, C. R., Deeprose, C., & Ganis, G. (2017). Undoing the past in order to lie in the present: Counterfactual thinking and deceptive communication. Cognition, 161, 66–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.003
  • Buck, M. L., & Miller, D. T. (1994). Reactions to incongruous negative life events. Social Justice Research, 7, 29–46. doi: 10.1007/BF02333821
  • Byrne, R. M. (2016). Counterfactual thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 135–157. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033249
  • Chandler, J., Mueller, P., & Paolacci, G. (2014). Nonnaïveté among Amazon Mechanical Turk workers: Consequences and solutions for behavioral researchers. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 112–130. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0365-7
  • Cialdini, R. B. (2003). Crafting normative messages to protect the environment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 105–109. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01242
  • Dixon, J. E., & Byrne, R. M. (2011). “If only” counterfactual thoughts about exceptional actions. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1317–1331. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0101-4
  • Epstein, S., Lipson, A., Holstein, C., & Huh, E. (1992). Irrational reactions to negative outcomes: evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 328–339. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.62.2.328
  • Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 168–192. doi: 10.1177/1088868308316091
  • Feldman, G. (2018). What is normal? Contrasting normality categories in the action-effect. Manuscript under review. Retrieved from https://osf.io/wmkpe/?view_only=4d181e034b974b59890650ed0f45d58b
  • Feldman, G., & Albarracín, D. (2017). Norm theory and the action-effect: The role of social norms in regret following action and inaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 111–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.07.009
  • Gavanski, I., & Wells, G. L. (1989). Counterfactual processing of normal and exceptional events. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 314–325. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(89)90025-5
  • Guttentag, R., & Ferrell, J. (2004). Reality compared with its alternatives: Age differences in judgments of regret and relief. Developmental Psychology, 40, 764–775. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.764
  • Hooker, C., Roese, N. J., & Park, S. (2000). Impoverished counterfactual thinking is associated with schizophrenia. Psychiatry, 63, 326–335. doi: 10.1080/00332747.2000.11024925
  • Hur, T., Roese, N. J., & Namkoong, J.-E. (2009). Regrets in the East and West: Role of intrapersonal versus interpersonal norms. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 12, 151–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2009.01275.x
  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.2.136
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201–210). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Koehler, J. J., & Prentice, R. A. (2003). A normality bias in legal decision making. Cornell Law Review, 88, 583–645.
  • Koonce, L., Miller, J., & Winchel, J. (2015). The effects of norms on investor reactions to derivative use. Contemporary Accounting Research, 32, 1529–1554. doi: 10.1111/1911-3846.12118
  • Kutscher, L., & Feldman, G. (2018). The impact of past behavior normality on regret: A meta-analysis. Manuscript under review. Retrieved from https://osf.io/xndtc/?view_only=1c0ed876e0604a5383d013b4dfd3f0dc
  • Landman, J. (1987). Regret and elation following action and inaction: Affective responses to positive versus negative outcomes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 524–536. doi: 10.1177/0146167287134009
  • Litman, L., Robinson, J., & Abberbock, T. (2017). TurkPrime.com: A versatile crowdsourcing data acquisition platform for the behavioral sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 433–442. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0727-z
  • Macrae, C. N. (1992). A tale of two curries: Counterfactual thinking and accident-related judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 84–87. doi: 10.1177/0146167292181012
  • Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Griffiths, R. J. (1993). Counterfactual thinking and the perception of criminal behaviour. British Journal of Psychology, 84, 221–226. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1993.tb02475.x
  • McEleney, A., & Byrne, R. M. (2006). Spontaneous counterfactual thoughts and causal explanations. Thinking & Reasoning, 12, 235–255. doi: 10.1080/13546780500317897
  • Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1986). Counterfactual thinking and victim compensation: A test of norm theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 513–519. doi: 10.1177/0146167286124014
  • Miller, D. T., Taylor, B., & Buck, M. L. (1991). Gender gaps: Who needs to be explained? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 5–12. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.5
  • Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & McFarland, C. (1989). When a coincidence is suspicious: The role of mental simulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 581–589. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.581
  • Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & McFarland, C. (1990). Counterfactual thinking and social perception: Thinking about what might have been. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 305–331.
  • N’gbala, A., & Branscombe, N. R. (1997). When does action elicit more regret than inaction and is counterfactual mutation the mediator of this effect? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 324–343. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1996.1322
  • Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 133–148. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.133
  • Roese, N. J., & Epstude, K. (2017). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking: New evidence, new challenges, new insights. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 1–79.
  • Roese, N. J., & Hur, T. (1997). Affective determinants of counterfactual thinking. Social Cognition, 15, 274–290. doi: 10.1521/soco.1997.15.4.274
  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1996). Counterfactuals, causal attributions, and the hindsight bias: A conceptual integration. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 197–227. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1996.0010
  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1997). Counterfactual thinking: The intersection of affect and function. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 1–59.
  • Turley, K. J., Sanna, L. J., & Reiter, R. L. (1995). Counterfactual thinking and perceptions of rape. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 285–303. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp1703_1
  • Wells, G. L., Taylor, B. R., & Turtle, J. W. (1987). The undoing of scenarios. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 421–430. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.421