788
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Attentional bias towards angry faces is moderated by the activation of a social processing mode in the general population

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 1317-1329 | Received 29 Mar 2018, Accepted 10 Dec 2018, Published online: 26 Dec 2018

References

  • Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 258–281. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.258
  • Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496. doi: 10.3758/BF03205306
  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1
  • Becker, D. V., Anderson, U. S., Mortensen, C. R., Neufeld, S. L., & Neel, R. (2011). The face in the crowd effect unconfounded: Happy faces, not angry faces, are more efficiently detected in single- and multiple-target visual search tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 637–659. doi: 10.1037/a0024060
  • Becker, S. I., Dutt, N., Vromen, J. M. G., & Horstmann, G. (2017). The capture of attention and gaze in the search for emotional photographic faces. Visual Cognition, 25, 241–261. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2017.1333182
  • Brosch, T., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Beyond fear: Rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychological Science, 19, 362–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02094.x
  • Calvo, M. G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2008). Detection of emotional faces: Salient physical features guide effective visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 471–494. doi: 10.1037/a0012771
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.
  • Cooper, R., & Langton, S. (2006). Attentional bias to angry faces using the dot-probe task? It depends when you look for it. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1321–1329. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.004
  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
  • Everaert, T., Spruyt, A., & De Houwer, J. (2013). On the malleability of automatic attentional biases: Effects of feature-specific attention allocation. Cognition & Emotion, 27, 385–400. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.712949
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
  • Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 847–858. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.847
  • Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (2006). Top-down modulation of preattentive processing: Testing the recovery account of contingent capture. Visual Cognition, 14, 445–465. doi: 10.1080/13506280500193545
  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.18.4.1030
  • Fox, E., Lester, V., Russo, R., Bowles, R. J., Pichler, A., & Dutton, K. (2000). Facial expressions of emotion: Are angry faces detected more efficiently? Cognition & Emotion, 14, 61–92. doi: 10.1080/026999300378996
  • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681–700. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.130.4.681 doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681
  • Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J. A., Joanisse, M. F., & Neufeld, R. W. J. (2008). Selective attention to threat versus reward: Meta-analysis and neural-network modeling of the dot-probe task. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 307–337. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.05.006
  • Glickman, M., & Lamy, D. (2018). Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional distractor faces is contingent on implicit attentional settings. Cognition & Emotion, 32, 303–314. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1301883
  • Hahn, S., & Gronlund, S. D. (2007). Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 159–165. doi: 10.3758/BF03194044
  • Hodsoll, S., Viding, E., & Lavie, N. (2011). Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional distractor faces. Emotion, 11, 346–353. doi: 10.1037/a0022771
  • Horstmann, G., & Bauland, A. (2006). Search asymmetries with real faces: Testing the anger-superiority effect. Emotion, 6, 193–207. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.193
  • Horstmann, G., Lipp, O. V., & Becker, S. I. (2012). Of toothy grins and angry snarls – open mouth displays contribute to efficiency gains in search for emotional faces. Journal of Vision, 12(7). doi: 10.1167/12.5.7
  • JASP Team. (2018). JASP (Version 0.9) [Computer software].
  • Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Juth, P., Lundqvist, D., Karlsson, A., & Öhman, A. (2005). Looking for foes and friends: Perceptual and emotional factors when finding a face in the crowd. Emotion, 5, 379–395. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.379
  • Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. (2007). What's new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36(Suppl. 1), 14.
  • Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1183–1192. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001
  • Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., & Spielberger, C. D. (1981). Das State-trait-Angstinventar (STAI): Theoretische Grundlagen und Handanweisung. Weinheim: Beltz.
  • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15
  • Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (1998). A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 539–560. doi: 10.1023/A:1018738019346
  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 809–848. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00063-1
  • Mogg, K., Holmes, A., Garner, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2008). Effects of threat cues on attentional shifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 656–667. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.011
  • Mogg, K., McNamara, J., Powys, M., Rawlinson, H., Seiffer, A., & Bradley, B. P. (2000). Selective attention to threat: A test of two cognitive models of anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 14, 375–399. doi: 10.1080/026999300378888
  • Moriya, J., Koster, E. H. W., & De Raedt, R. (2014). The influence of working memory on visual search for emotional facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1874–1890. doi: 10.1037/a0037295
  • Mulckhuyse, M., & Crombez, G. (2014). Disentangling attention from action in the emotional spatial cueing task. Cognition & Emotion, 28, 1223–1241. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.878688
  • Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315–330. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.15.2.315
  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.466 doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466
  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.108.3.483 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483
  • Petrova, K., Wentura, D., & Bermeitinger, C. (2013). What happens during the stimulus onset asynchrony in the dot-probe task? Exploring the role of eye movements in the assessment of attentional biases. PLoS ONE, 8, e76335. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076335
  • Pinkham, A. E., Griffin, M., Baron, R., Sasson, N. J., & Gur, R. C. (2010). The face in the crowd effect: Anger superiority when using real faces and multiple identities. Emotion, 10, 141–146. doi: 10.1037/a0017387
  • Pool, E., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., & Sander, D. (2016). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 79–106. doi: 10.1037/bul0000026
  • Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160
  • Puls, S., & Rothermund, K. (2017). Attending to emotional expressions: No evidence for automatic capture in the dot-probe task. Cognition & Emotion. Advance Online Publication. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1314932
  • Savage, R. A., Lipp, O. V., Craig, B. M., Becker, S. I., & Horstmann, G. (2013). In search of the emotional face: Anger versus happiness superiority in visual search. Emotion, 13, 758–768. doi: 10.1037/a0031970
  • Schönbrodt, F. D., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Zehetleitner, M., & Perugini, M. (2017). Sequential hypothesis testing with Bayes factors: Efficiently testing mean differences. Psychological Methods, 22, 322–339. doi: 10.1037/met0000061
  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Hermans, D. (2009). Modulation of automatic semantic priming by feature-specific attention allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 37–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.03.004
  • Stevens, S., Rist, F., & Gerlach, A. L. (2011). Eye movement assessment in individuals with social phobia: Differential usefulness for varying presentation times? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42, 219–224. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.11.001
  • Sussman, T. J., Jin, J., & Mohanty, A. (2016). Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in anxiety. Biological Psychology, 121, 160–172. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
  • Theeuwes, J. (2004). Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 65–70. doi: 10.3758/BF03206462
  • Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., … Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006
  • Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., & van Damme, S. (2013). Competing for attentional priority: Temporary goals versus threats. Emotion, 13, 587–598. doi: 10.1037/a0027204
  • Vromen, J. M. G., Lipp, O. V., Remington, R. W., & Becker, S. I. (2016). Threat captures attention, but not automatically: Top-down goals modulate attentional orienting to threat distractors. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1142-3
  • Vromen, J. M. G., Lipp, O. V., & Remington, R. W. (2015). The spider does not always win the fight for attention: Disengagement from threat is modulated by goal set. Cognition & Emotion, 29, 1185–1196. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.969198
  • Wagenmakers, E.-J., Wetzels, R., Borsboom, D., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2011). Why psychologists must change the way they analyze their data: The case of psi: Comment on Bem (2011). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 426–432. doi: 10.1037/a0022790
  • Weierich, M. R., Treat, T. A., & Hollingworth, A. (2008). Theories and measurement of visual attentional processing in anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 985–1018. doi: 10.1080/02699930701597601
  • Williams, J. M., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorder. Chichester: Wiley.
  • Wirth, B. E., & Wentura, D. (2018a). Attentional bias to threat in the general population is contingent on target competition, not on attentional control settings. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 975–988. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1307864
  • Wirth, B. E., & Wentura, D. (2018b). Furious snarling: Teeth-exposure and anxiety-related attentional bias towards angry faces. PLoS ONE, 13, e0207695. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207695
  • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0. A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238. doi: 10.3758/BF03200774
  • Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 3–47. doi: 10.1080/02699930903205698

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.