330
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Brief Articles

The effect of emotional distraction on hyper-binding in young and older adults

, , &
Pages 839-847 | Received 17 May 2019, Accepted 12 Sep 2019, Published online: 26 Sep 2019

References

  • Amer, T., Campbell, K. L., & Hasher, L. (2016). Cognitive control as a double-edged sword. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 905–915. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.002
  • Armony, J., & Dolan, R. (2002). Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned stimuli: An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 817–826. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00178-6
  • Biss, R. K., Campbell, K. L., & Hasher, L. (2013). Interference from previous distraction disrupts older adults’ memory. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 68, 558–561. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs074
  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW). Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida.
  • Campbell, K. L., & Hasher, L. (2018). Hyper-binding only apparent under fully implicit test conditions. Psychology and Aging, 33, 176–181. doi: 10.1037/pag0000216
  • Campbell, K. L., Hasher, L., & Thomas, R. C. (2010). Hyper-binding: A unique age effect. Psychological Science, 21, 399–405. doi: 10.1177/0956797609359910
  • Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life-span theory of socioemotional selectivity. Psychological Science, 4, 151–156.
  • Carstensen, L. L., & DeLiema, M. (2018). The positivity effect: A negativity bias in youth fades with age. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.009
  • Gallant, S. N., & Yang, L. (2014). Positivity effect in source attributions of arousal-matched emotional and non-emotional words during item-based directed forgetting. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1334. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01334
  • Giesen, C., & Rothermund, K. (2011). Affective matching moderates S–R binding. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 342–350. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.482765
  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 193–225). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.007
  • Isaacowitz, D. M., Toner, K., Goren, D., & Wilson, H. R. (2008). Looking while unhappy: Mood-congruent gaze in young adults, positive gaze in older adults. Psychological Science, 19, 848–853. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02167.x
  • Katzman, R., Brown, T., Fuld, P., Peck, A., Schechter, R., & Schimmel, H. (1983). Validation of a short orientation-memory concentration test of cognitive impairment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 734–739. doi: 10.1176/ajp.140.6.734
  • Kensinger, E. A. (2008). Age differences in memory for arousing and nonarousing emotional words. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63, P13–P18. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.1.P13
  • Kim, S., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (2007). Aging and a benefit of distractibility. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 301–305. doi: 10.3758/BF03194068
  • Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490. doi: 10.3758/BF03210951
  • Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the depression anxiety Stress Scales (2nd ed.). Sydney: Psychology Foundation.
  • MacLeod, C., & McLaughlin, K. (1995). Implicit and explicit memory bias in anxiety: A conceptual replication. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)E0004-3
  • Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object-based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 33–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00028.x
  • Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005
  • Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory performance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1170–1187.
  • Paivio, A. (1965). Abstractness, imagery, and meaningfulness in paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 32–38. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(65)80064-0
  • Pehlivanoglu, D., & Verhaeghen, P. (2019). Now you feel it, now you don't: Motivated attention to emotional content is modulated by age and task demands. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 1–18.
  • Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 160–166. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006
  • Peters, J., & Daum, I. (2008). Differential effects of normal aging on recollection of concrete and abstract words. Neuropsychology, 22, 255–261. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.2.255
  • Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380
  • Reed, A. E., Chan, L., & Mikels, J. A. (2014). Meta-analysis of the age-related positivity effect: Age differences in preferences for positive over negative information. Psychology and Aging, 29, 1–15. doi: 10.1037/a0035194
  • Shipley, W. C. (1946). Institute of living scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.
  • Thomas, R. C., & Hasher, L. (2006). The influence of emotional valence on age differences in early processing and memory. Psychology and Aging, 21, 821–825. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.821
  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
  • Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.
  • Weeks, J. C., Biss, R. K., Murphy, K. J., & Hasher, L. (2016). Face-name learning in older adults: A benefit of hyper-binding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1559–1565. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1003-z

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.