1,129
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Differential effects of abstract and concrete processing on the reactivity of basic and self-conscious emotions

, , &
Pages 593-606 | Received 01 Apr 2020, Accepted 04 Nov 2020, Published online: 22 Nov 2020

References

  • Agerström, J., & Björklund, F. (2013). Why people with an eye toward the future are more moral: The role of abstract thinking. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(4), 373–381. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2013.803967
  • Aldao, A. (2013). The future of emotion regulation research: Capturing context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 155–172. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612459518
  • Alper, S. (2018). An abstract mind is a principled one: Abstract mindset increases consistency in responses to political attitude scales. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 77, 89–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.04.008
  • Amit, E., Algom, D., & Trope, Y. (2009). Distance-dependent processing of pictures and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 400–415. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015835
  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 1(2), 97–111. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.12
  • Devlin, J., Chang, M. W., Lee, K., & Toutanova, K. (2018). Bert: Pre-training of deep bidirectional transformers for language understanding. arXiv Preprint ArXiv:1810.04805.
  • Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411068
  • Eyal, T., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2008). Judging near and distant virtue and vice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1204–1209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.03.012
  • Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43(5), 349–358. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.43.5.349
  • Fujita, K., Eyal, T., Chaiken, S., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2008). Influencing attitudes toward near and distant objects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 562–572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.005
  • Giacomantonio, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., Shalvi, S., Sligte, D. J., & Leder, S. (2010). Psychological distance boosts value-behavior correspondence in ultimatum bargaining and integrative negotiation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(5), 824–829. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.001
  • Gilead, M., Katzir, M., Eyal, T., & Liberman, N. (2016). Neural correlates of processing “self-conscious” vs. “basic” emotions. Neuropsychologia, 81, 207–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.009
  • Gong, H., & Medin, D. L. (2012). Construal levels and moral judgment: Some complications. Judgment and Decision Making, 7, 628–638.
  • Gross, J. J. (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. Guilford Press.
  • Hart, D., & Karmel, M. P. (1996). Self-awareness and self-knowledge in humans, apes, and monkeys. In A. E. Russon, & K. A. Bard (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 325–347). Cambridge University Press.
  • Harvey, A., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders: A transdiagnostic approach to research and treatment. Oxford University Press.
  • Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., & Dalgleish, T. (2008). The causal effect of mental imagery on emotion assessed using picture-word cues. Emotion, 8(3), 395–409. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.395
  • Houser-Marko, L., & Sheldon, K. M. (2008). Eyes on the prize or nose to the grindstone? The effects of level of goal evaluation on mood and motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(11), 1556–1569. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208322618
  • Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Katzir, M., & Eyal, T. (2013). When stepping outside the self is not enough: A self-distanced perspective reduces the experience of basic but not of self-conscious emotions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(6), 1089–1092. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.07.006
  • Katzir, M., Eyal, T., Meiran, N., & Kessler, Y. (2010). Imagined positive emotions and inhibitory control: The differentiated effect of pride versus happiness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(5), 1314–1320. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020120
  • Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 13(5), 467–480. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379140
  • Koenigsberg, H. W., Fan, J., Ochsner, K. N., Liu, X., Guise, K. G., Pizzarello, S., Dorantes, C., Guerreri, S., Tecuta, L., Goodman, M., & New, A. (2009). Neural correlates of the use of psychological distancing to regulate responses to negative social cues: A study of patients with borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66(9), 854–863. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.010
  • Kross, E., Ayduk, Ö, & Mischel, W. (2005). When asking “why” does not hurt distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative emotions. Psychological Science, 16(9), 709–715. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01600.x
  • Kyung, E. J., Menon, G., & Trope, Y. (2010). Reconstruction of things past: Why do some memories feel so close and others so far away? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 217–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.003
  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford University Press.
  • Leary, M. R. (2007). Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 317–344. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085658
  • Lee, D. S., Orvell, A., Briskin, J., Shrapnell, T., Gelman, S. A., Ayduk, O., Ybarra, O., Kross, E. (2020). When chatting about negative experiences helps – and when it hurts: Distinguishing adaptive versus maladaptive social support in computer-mediated communication. Emotion, 20(3), 368–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000555
  • Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2014). Traversing psychological distance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 364–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.001
  • Moran, T., Bornstein, O., & Eyal, T. (2019). The level of construal involved in the elicitation of core versus moral disgust. Emotion. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000709
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x
  • Pennebaker, J. W., & Chung, C. K. (2007). Expressive writing, emotional upheavals, and health. In H. Friedman, & R. Silver (Eds.), Handbook of Health Psychology (pp. 263–284). Oxford University Press.
  • Rimes, K. A., & Watkins, E. (2005). The effects of self-focused rumination on global negative self-judgements in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(12), 1673–1681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.12.002
  • Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2008). Disgust. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 757–776). Guilford Press.
  • Sheppes, G., & Meiran, N. (2007). Better late than never? On the dynamics of online regulation of sadness using distraction and cognitive reappraisal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(11), 1518–1532. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207305537
  • Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of time perspective, affect, and mode of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(6), 1460–1469. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.49.6.1460
  • Takano, K., & Tanno, Y. (2010). Concreteness of thinking and self-focus. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 419–425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.11.010
  • Tamir, M., & Ford, B. Q. (2012). When feeling bad is expected to be good: Emotion regulation and outcome expectancies in social conflicts. Emotion, 12(4), 807–816. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024443
  • Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychological Inquiry, 15(2), 103–125. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1502_01
  • Trope, Y., Ledgerwood, A., Liberman, N., & Fujita, K. (2020). Regulatory scope and its mental and social support. Perspective on Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620950691.
  • Uusberg, A., Taxer, J. L., Yih, J., Uusberg, H., & Gross, J. J. (2019). Reappraising reappraisal. Emotion Review, 11(4), 267–282. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073919862617
  • Valenti, G., Libby, L. K., & Eibach, R. P. (2011). Looking back with regret: Visual perspective in memory images differentially affects regret for actions and inactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(4), 730–737. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.02.008
  • Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 660–671. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.660
  • Vrielynck, N., & Philippot, P. (2009). Regulating emotion during imaginal exposure to social anxiety: Impact of the specificity of information processing. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40(2), 274–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.12.006
  • Watkins, E. (2011). Dysregulation in level of goal and action identification across psychological disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(2), 260–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.004
  • Watkins, E. R., Moberly, N. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Processing mode causally influences emotional reactivity: Distinct effects of abstract versus concrete construal on emotional response. Emotion, 8(3), 364–378. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.364
  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Explaining away: A model of affective adaptation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 370–386. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00085.x
  • Žeželj, I. L., & Jokić, B. R. (2014). Replication of experiments evaluating impact of psychological distance on moral judgment. Social Psychology, 45(3), 223–231. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000188

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.