226
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Contextual positivity-familiarity effects are unaffected by known moderators of misattribution

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 636-648 | Received 09 Jun 2020, Accepted 26 Nov 2020, Published online: 10 Dec 2020

References

  • Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychological Bulletin, 106(2), 265–289. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.265
  • Claypool, H. M., Hall, C. E., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2008). Positive mood, attribution, and the illusion of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 721–728. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.05.001
  • Corneille, O., Monin, B., & Pleyers, G. (2005). Is positivity a cue or a response option? Warm glow vs. evaluative matching in the familiarity for attractive and not-so-attractive faces. . Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(4), 431–437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.08.004
  • Crandall, C. S., & Sherman, J. W. (2016). On the scientific superiority of conceptual replications for scientific progress. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66, 93–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.002
  • Duke, D., Fiacconi, C. M., & Köhler, S. (2014). Parallel effects of processing fluency and positive affect on familiarity-based recognition decisions for faces. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 328. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00328
  • Ebner, N. C., Riediger, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). FACES - A database of facial expressions in young, middle-aged, and older women and men: Development and validation. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 351–362. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.42.1.351
  • Ecker, Y., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2019a). Applicability increases the effect of misattribution on judgment. Cognition and Emotion, 33(4), 709–721. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1498322
  • Ecker, Y., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2019b). Conceptual overlap between stimuli increases misattribution of internal experience. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 83, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.03.006
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146
  • Garcia-Marques, T., Mackie, D. M., Claypool, H. M., & Garcia-Marques, L. (2004). Positivity can cue familiarity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(5), 585–593. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203262856
  • Gellatly, A., Banton, P., & Woods, C. (1995). Salience and awareness in the Jacoby–Whitehouse effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(5), 1374–1379. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.5.1374
  • Hazlett, C. J., & Berinsky, A. J. (2018). Stress-testing the affect misattribution procedure: Heterogeneous control of affect misattribution procedure effects under incentives. British Journal of Social Psychology, 57(1), 61–74. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12217
  • Housley, M. K., Claypool, H. M., Garcia-Marques, T., & Mackie, D. M. (2010). “We” are familiar but “It” is not: Ingroup pronouns trigger feelings of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 114–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.011
  • Jacoby, L. L., & Kelley, C. M. (1987). Unconscious influences of memory for a prior event. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13(3), 314–336. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167287133003
  • Jacoby, L. L., & Whitehouse, K. (1989). An illusion of memory: False recognition influenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(2), 126–135. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.118.2.126
  • JASP Team. (2019). JASP (Version 0.11.1) [Computer software] https://jasp-stats.org/faq/how-do-i-cite-jasp/.
  • Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.
  • Jones, C. R., Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2009). Implicit misattribution as a mechanism underlying evaluative conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 933–948. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014747
  • Kelley, H. H. (1971). Attribution in social interaction. General Learning Press.
  • Krieglmeyer, R., & Sherman, J. W. (2012). Disentangling stereotype activation and stereotype application in the stereotype misperception task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(2), 205–224. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028764
  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). The international affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
  • Langner, O., Dotsch, R., Bijlstra, G., Wigboldus, D. H., Hawk, S. T., & Van Knippenberg, A. D. (2010). Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Cognition and Emotion, 24(8), 1377–1388. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903485076
  • Lee, M. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2013). Bayesian cognitive modeling: A practical course. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139087759
  • Loersch, C., & Payne, B. K. (2011). The situated inference model: An integrative account of the effects of primes on perception, behavior, and motivation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 234–252. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611406921
  • Ma, D., Correll, J., & Wittenbrink, B. (2015). The Chicago Face Database: A free stimulus set of faces and norming data. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 1122–1135. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0532-5
  • Maxwell, S. E., Lau, M. Y., & Howard, G. S. (2015). Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? American Psychologist, 70(6), 487–498. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039400
  • Monahan, J. L., Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Subliminal mere exposure: Specific, general, and diffuse effects. Psychological Science, 11(6), 462–466. https://doi.org/10.1111%2F1467-9280.00289
  • Monin, B. (2003). The warm glow heuristic: When liking leads to familiarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1035–1048. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1035
  • Oikawa, M., Aarts, H., & Oikawa, H. (2011). There is a fire burning in my heart: The role of causal attribution in affect transfer. Cognition and Emotion, 25(1), 156–163. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931003680061
  • Olszanowski, M., Pochwatko, G., Kukliński, K., Ścibor-Rylski, M., Lewicki, P., & Ohme, R. (2015). Warsaw set of emotional facial expression pictures: A validation study of facial display photographs. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1516. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01516
  • Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 105(32), 11087–11092. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0805664105
  • Palan, S., & Schitter, C. (2018). Prolific.ac—A subject pool for online experiments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 17, 22–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbef.2017.12.004
  • Payne, B. K., Cheng, S. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 277–293. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277
  • Peer, E., Brandimarte, L., Samat, S., & Acquisti, A. (2017). Beyond the Turk: Alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 153–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.01.006
  • Phaf, H., & Rotteveel, M. (2012). Affective monitoring: A generic mechanism for affect elicitation. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, Article 47. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00047
  • Phaf, R. H., & Rotteveel, M. (2005). Affective modulation of recognition bias. Emotion, 5(3), 309–318. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.309
  • Rotteveel, M., & Phaf, R. H. (2007). Mere exposure in reverse: Mood and motion modulate memory bias. Cognition and Emotion, 21(6), 1323–1346. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701438319
  • Ruys, K. I., Aarts, H., Papies, E. K., Oikawa, M., & Oikawa, H. (2012). Perceiving an exclusive cause of affect prevents misattribution. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(2), 1009–1015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.03.002
  • Van der Schalk, J., Hawk, S. T., Fischer, A. H., & Doosje, B. J. (2011). Moving faces, looking places: The Amsterdam Dynamic Facial Expressions Set (ADFES). Emotion, 11(4), 907–920. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023853
  • Verwijmeren, T., Karremans, J. C., Bernritter, S. F., Stroebe, W., & Wigboldus, D. H. (2013). Warning: You are being primed! The effect of a warning on the impact of subliminal ads. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(6), 1124–1129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.06.010
  • Weil, R., Palma, T. A., & Gawronski, B. (2017). At the boundaries of misattribution: Does positivity influence judgments of familiarity in the affect misattribution procedure? Experimental Psychology, 64(6), 369–386. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000379
  • Weil, R., Palma, T. A., & Gawronski, B. (2020). When does contextual positivity influence judgments of familiarity? Investigating moderators of the positivity-familiarity effect. Social Cognition, 38(2), 119–145. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2020.38.2.119
  • Westerman, D. L. (2008). Relative fluency and illusions of recognition memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(6), 1196–1200. https://doi.org/10.3758/pbr.15.6.1196
  • Westerman, D. L., Lloyd, M. E., & Miller, J. K. (2002). The attribution of perceptual fluency in recognition memory: The role of expectation. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(4), 607–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0749-596x(02)00022-0
  • White, G. L., & Knight, T. D. (1984). Misattribution of arousal and attraction: Effects of salience of explanations of arousal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20(1), 55–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(84)90012-x
  • Whittlesea, B. W., & Leboe, J. P. (2003). Two fluency heuristics (and how to tell them apart). Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 62–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0749-596x(03)00009-3
  • Whittlesea, B. W., & Williams, L. D. (1998). Why do strangers feel familiar, but friends don't? A discrepancy-attribution account of feelings of familiarity. Acta Psychologica, 98(2–3), 141–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(97)00040-1
  • Whittlesea, B. W., & Williams, L. D. (2001). The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: I. The heuristic basis of feelings and familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(1), 3–13. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.27.1.3
  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2000). The source of feelings of familiarity: The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(3), 547–565. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.26.3.547
  • Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In J. Musch, & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189–217). Erlbaum.
  • Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025848

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.