2,067
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
New Theory Section

A perceptual control theory of emotional action

ORCID Icon

References

  • Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Damasio, A. (2008). Embodied semantics for actions: Findings from functional brain imaging. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1), 35–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.012
  • Badets, A., Koch, I., & Philipp, A. M. (2016). A review of ideomotor approaches to perception, cognition, action, and language: Advancing a cultural recycling hypothesis. Psychological Research, 80(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0643-8
  • Barrett, L. F., & Russell, J. A. (2014). The psychological construction of emotion. Guilford Publications.
  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577–660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002149
  • Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., DeWall, C. N., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 167–203. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868307301033
  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1–3), 7–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3
  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275(5304), 1293–1295. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.275.5304.1293
  • Beckers, T., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2002). Automatic integration of non-perceptual action effect features: The case of the associative affective Simon effect. Psychological Research, 66(3), 166–173. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-002-0090-9
  • Berridge, K. C. (2018). Evolving concepts of emotion and motivation. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1647. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01647
  • Berridge, K. C., & Valenstein, E. S. (1991). What psychological process mediates feeding evoked by electrical stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus? Behavioral Neuroscience, 105(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.105.1.3
  • Bolles, R. C. (1970). Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychological Review, 77(1), 32–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0028589
  • Bonini, L., Rotunno, C., Arcuri, E., & Gallese, V. (2022). Mirror neurons 30 years later: Implications and applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(9), 767–781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.003
  • Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2003). The devaluation effect: Activating a need devalues unrelated objects. Journal of Consumer Research, 29(4), 463–473. https://doi.org/10.1086/346243
  • Bromberg-Martin, E. S., Matsumoto, M., & Hikosaka, O. (2010). Dopamine in motivational control: Rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron, 68(5), 815–834. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.022
  • Cartoni, E., Balleine, B., & Baldassarre, G. (2016). Appetitive Pavlovian-instrumental Transfer: A review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 829–848. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.020
  • Church, R. M. (1963). The varied effects of punishment on behavior. Psychological Review, 70(5), 369–402. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046499
  • Craig, A. D. (2015). How do you feel? An interoceptive moment with your neurobiological self. Princeton University Press.
  • Critchley, H. D., & Garfinkel, S. N. (2017). Interoception and emotion. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 7–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.020
  • Cunningham, W. A., Dunfield, K. A., & Stillman, P. E. (2013). Emotional states from affective dynamics. Emotion Review, 5(4), 344–355. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913489749
  • Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science, 329(5987), 47–50. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1188595
  • Cuthbert, B. N., Vrana, S. R., & Bradley, M. M. (1991). Imagery: Function and physiology. In J. R. Jennings, M. Ackles, & G. H. Coles (Eds.), Advances in psychophysiology: A research annual (Vol. 4) (pp. 1–42). Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
  • Damasio, A. (1989). The brain binds entities and events by multiregional activation from convergence zones. Neural Computation, 1(1), 123–132. https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.1989.1.1.123
  • Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness (Vol. xiv). Harcourt College Publishers.
  • Damasio, A., & Carvalho, G. B. (2013). The nature of feelings: Evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(2), 143–152. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3403
  • Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotion in man and animal. William Clows & Sons.
  • de Wit, S., & Dickinson, A. (2009). Associative theories of goal-directed behaviour: A case for animal–human translational models. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 73(4), 463–476. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0230-6
  • Dignath, D., Kiesel, A., Frings, C., & Pastötter, B. (2020). Electrophysiological evidence for action-effect prediction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(6), 1148–1155. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000707
  • Eder, A. B., Erle, T. M., & Kunde, W. (2020). Reward strengthens action–effect binding. Motivation Science, 6(3), 297–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000153
  • Eder, A. B., & Hommel, B. (2013). Anticipatory control of approach and avoidance: An ideomotor approach. Emotion Review, 5(3), 275–279. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913477505
  • Eder, A. B., & Klauer, K. C. (2007). Common valence coding in action and evaluation: Affective blindness towards response-compatible stimuli. Cognition & Emotion, 21(6), 1297–1322. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701438277
  • Eder, A. B., & Klauer, K. C. (2009). A common-coding account of the bidirectional evaluation–behavior link. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(2), 218–235. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015220
  • Eder, A. B., Mitschke, V., & Gollwitzer, M. (2020). What stops revenge taking? Effects of observed emotional reactions on revenge seeking. Aggressive Behavior, 46(4), 305–316. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21890
  • Eder, A. B., Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (2012). The structure of affective action representations: Temporal binding of affective response codes. Psychological Research, 76(1), 111–118. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0327-6
  • Eder, A. B., Pfister, R., Dignath, D., & Hommel, B. (2017). Anticipatory affect during action preparation: Evidence from backward compatibility in dual-task performance. Cognition and Emotion, 31(6), 1211–1224. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1208151
  • Eder, A. B., & Rothermund, K. (2008). When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 262–281. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.262
  • Eder, A. B., Rothermund, K., De Houwer, J., & Hommel, B. (2015). Directive and incentive functions of affective action consequences: An ideomotor approach. Psychological Research, 79(4), 630–649. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0590-4
  • Ferguson, M. J. (2007). On the automatic evaluation of end-states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(4), 596–611. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.596
  • Flaherty, C. F. (1996). Incentive relativity. Cambridge University Press.
  • Frijda, N. H. (2004). Emotions and action. In A. Fischer, A. S. R. Manstead, & N. H. Frijda (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium (pp. 158–173). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511806582.010.
  • Frijda, N. H., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Rietveld, E. (2014). Impulsive action: Emotional impulses and their control. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 518. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00518
  • Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., & Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychology, 104(Supplement C), 65–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004
  • Goldie, P. (2002). Emotions, feelings and intentionality. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 1(3), 235–254. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021306500055
  • Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77(2), 73–99. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0028689
  • Guthrie, E. R. (1935). The psychology of learning. Harper.
  • Harleß, E. (1861). Der apparat des willens. Zeitschrift Für Philosophie Und Philosophische Kritik, 38(2), 50–73.
  • Holmes, N. M., Marchand, A. R., & Coutureau, E. (2010). Pavlovian to instrumental transfer: A neurobehavioural perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(8), 1277–1295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.007
  • Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 494–500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.007
  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–937. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01000103
  • Hommel, B., & Wiers, R. W. (2017). Towards a unitary approach to human action control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(12), 940–949. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.009
  • Izard, C. E. (2010). The many meanings/aspects of emotion: Definitions, functions, activation, and regulation. Emotion Review, 2(4), 363–370. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910374661
  • James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind; A Quarterly Review of Psychology and Philosophy, 9, 188–205. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/os-IX.34.188
  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology, Vol I. Henry Holt and Co.
  • Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor cognition: What actions tell the self (Vol. 42). Oxford University Press.
  • Keele, S. W. (1968). Movement control in skilled motor performance. Psychological Bulletin, 70(6, Pt.1), 387–403. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026739
  • Kim, H., Shimojo, S., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2006). Is avoiding an aversive outcome rewarding? Neural substrates of avoidance learning in the human brain. PLoS Biology, 4(8), e233. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040233
  • Klauer, K. C., Eder, A. B., Greenwald, A. G., & Abrams, R. L. (2007). Priming of semantic classifications by novel subliminal prime words. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(1), 63–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.12.002
  • Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 394–421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010
  • Kühn, S., Elsner, B., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2009). Busy doing nothing: Evidence for nonaction—Effect binding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(3), 542–549. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.3.542
  • Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88(2), 223–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00023-4
  • Kunde, W., Schmidts, C., Wirth, R., & Herbort, O. (2017). Action effects are coded as transitions from current to future stimulation: Evidence from compatibility effects in tracking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(3), 477–486. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000311
  • Lambie, J. A., & Marcel, A. J. (2002). Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: A theoretical framework. Psychological Review, 109(2), 219–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.2.219
  • Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16(6), 495–512. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1979.tb01511.x
  • LeDoux, J. E. (2014). Coming to terms with fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(8), 2871–2878. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1400335111
  • Levy, D. J., & Glimcher, P. W. (2012). The root of all value: A neural common currency for choice. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22(6), 1027–1038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.06.001
  • Lewin, K. (1926). Vorsatz, Wille und Bedürfnis [Intention, will, and need]. Psychologische Forschung, 7(1), 330–385. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02424365
  • Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische psychologie oder physiologie der seele. Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung.
  • Mansell, W.2020). The interdisciplinary handbook of perceptual control theory: Living control systems IV. Academic Press.
  • Marken, R. S., & Mansell, W. (2013). Perceptual control as a unifying concept in psychology. Review of General Psychology, 17(2), 190–195. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032933
  • McAllister, W. R., & McAllister, D. E. (1992). Fear determines the effectiveness of a feedback stimulus in aversively motivated instrumental learning. Learning and Motivation, 23(1), 99–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(92)90025-H
  • Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15(4), 243–247. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00659.x
  • Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77(3), 249–259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0435-y
  • Mertens, G., Krypotos, A.-M., & Engelhard, I. M. (2020). A review on mental imagery in fear conditioning research 100 years since the ‘Little Albert’ study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 126, 103556. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2020.103556
  • Milner, D., & Goodale, M. (2006). The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press.
  • Moors, A. (2022). Demystifying emotions: A typology of theories in psychology and philosophy. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781107588882.
  • Moors, A., & Fischer, M. (2019). Demystifying the role of emotion in behaviour: Toward a goal-directed account. Cognition and Emotion, 33(1), 94–100. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1510381
  • Moseley, R., Carota, F., Hauk, O., Mohr, B., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). A role for the motor system in binding abstract emotional meaning. Cerebral Cortex, 22(7), 1634–1647. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr238
  • Mostofsky, S. H., & Simmonds, D. J. (2008). Response inhibition and response selection: Two sides of the same coin. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(5), 751–761. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20500
  • Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Hari, R., & Hietanen, J. K. (2014). Bodily maps of emotions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(2), 646–651. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1321664111
  • Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(1), 30–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004
  • Papies, E. K., Barsalou, L. W., & Rusz, D. (2020). Understanding desire for food and drink: A grounded-cognition approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(2), 193–198. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420904958
  • Pfister, R. (2019). Effect-based action control with body-related effects: Implications for empirical approaches to ideomotor action control. Psychological Review, 126(1), 153–161. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000140
  • Pool, E., Sennwald, V., Delplanque, S., Brosch, T., & Sander, D. (2016). Measuring wanting and liking from animals to humans: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 63, 124–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.006
  • Poppa, T., & Bechara, A. (2018). The somatic marker hypothesis: Revisiting the role of the ‘body-loop’ in decision-making. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 61–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.10.007
  • Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Aldine Publishing Company.
  • Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann, & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches (pp. 167–201). Springer-Verlag.
  • Prinz, W., de Maeght, S., & Knuf, L. (2004). Intention in action. In G. Humphreys, & J. Riddoch (Eds.), Attention in action (pp. 93–108). Psychology Press.
  • Pulvermüller, F., & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active perception: Sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(5), 351–360. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2811
  • Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2017). Emotion in action: A predictive processing perspective and theoretical synthesis. Emotion Review, 9(4), 319–325. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916661765
  • Rolls, E. T. (2005). Emotion explained. Oxford University Press.
  • Sander, D., & Nummenmaa, L. (2021). Reward and emotion: An affective neuroscience approach. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 39, 161–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.03.016
  • Scherer, K. R. (2009). The dynamic architecture of emotion: Evidence for the component process model. Cognition & Emotion, 23(7), 1307–1351. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930902928969
  • Schulkin, J., & Sterling, P. (2019). Allostasis: A brain-centered, predictive mode of physiological regulation. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(10), 740–752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2019.07.010
  • Schultz, W. (2007). Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(5), 203–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.007
  • Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139173452.
  • Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 943–974. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020541
  • Singer, W. (2001). Consciousness and the binding problem. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 929(1), 123–146. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05712.x
  • Strohmaier, N., & Veling, H. (2019). Bypassing the gatekeeper: Incidental negative cues stimulate choices with negative outcomes. Cognition and Emotion, 33(5), 1059–1066. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1523136
  • Tamir, M. (2016). Why do people regulate their emotions? A taxonomy of motives in emotion regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(3), 199–222. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868315586325
  • Taquet, M., Quoidbach, J., Montjoye, Y.-A. d., Desseilles, M., & Gross, J. J. (2016). Hedonism and the choice of everyday activities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(35), 9769–9773. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1519998113
  • Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness: Vol. I. The positive affects. Springer.
  • Warlow, S. M., & Berridge, K. C. (2021). Incentive motivation: ‘Wanting’ roles of central amygdala circuitry. Behavioural Brain Research, 411, 113376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113376
  • Waszak, F., Cardoso-Leite, P., & Hughes, G. (2012). Action effect anticipation: Neurophysiological basis and functional consequences. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(2), 943–959. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.004
  • Winkielman, P., & Berridge, K. C. (2004). Unconscious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(3), 120–123. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00288.x
  • Wolpert, D. M., & Landy, M. S. (2012). Motor control is decision-making. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22(6), 996–1003. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.05.003
  • Yarali, A., Niewalda, T., Chen, Y., Tanimoto, H., Duerrnagel, S., & Gerber, B. (2008). Pain relief’ learning in fruit flies. Animal Behaviour, 76(4), 1173–1185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.025