371
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Orthographic and Morphological Masked Priming Effects in Arabic: Evidence from Lexical Decision Task in Children with and Without Dyslexia

, , &
Pages 243-268 | Received 13 Nov 2018, Accepted 29 Apr 2019, Published online: 14 May 2019

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Awwad, J. S. (2004). Morphological structures in visual word recognition: The case of Arabic. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(3), 321–336. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2004.00235.x
  • Acha, J., & Perea, M. (2008). The effects of length and transposed-letter similarity in lexical decision: Evidence with beginning, intermediate, and adult readers. British Journal of Psychology, 99(2), 245–264. doi: 10.1348/000712607X224478
  • Araújo, S., Faísca, L., Bramão, I., Petersson, K. M., & Reis, A. (2014). Lexical and phonological processes in dyslexic readers: Evidence from a visual lexical decision task. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 20(1), 38–53. doi: 10.1002/dys.1461
  • Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2015). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(2), 12–28. doi: 10.21500/20112084.807
  • Bentin, S., Deouell, L., & Soroker, N. (1999). Selective visual streaming in face recognition: Evidence from developmental prosopagnosia. NeuroReport, 10(4), 823–827. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199903170-00029
  • Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (2001). Linguistic theory and psychological reality: A reply to Boudelaa & Marslen-Willson. Cognition, 81(1), 113–118. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00120-2
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2001). Morphological units in the Arabic mental lexicon. Cognition, 81(1), 65–92. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00119-6
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2004a). Abstract morphemes and lexical representation: The CV-skeleton in Arabic. Cognition, 92(3), 271–303. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.08.003
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2004b). Allomorphic variation in Arabic: Implications for lexical processing and representation. Brain and Language, 90(1–3), 106–116. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00424-3
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2005). Discontinuous morphology in time: Incremental masked priming in Arabic. Language & Cognitive Processes, 20, 207–260. doi: 10.1080/01690960444000106
  • Bouma, A., Mulder, J., & Lindeboom, J. (1996). Neuropsychologische diagnostiek: Handboek [Neuropsychological assessment: Manual]. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
  • Bowey, J. A., & Muller, D. (2005). Phonological recoding and rapid orthographic learning in third-graders´ silent reading: A critical test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92(3), 203–219. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.06.005
  • Caramazza, A., Laudanna, A., & Romani, C. (1988). Lexical access and inflectional morphology. Cognition, 28(3), 297–332.
  • Carreiras, M., Perea, M., & Mallouh, R. A. (2012). Priming of abstract letter representaions may be universal: The case of Arabic. Psychonomis Bulletin and Review, 19(4), 685–690.
  • Casalis, S., Dusautoir, M., Colé, P., & Ducrot, S. (2009). Morphological effects in children word reading: A priming study in fourth graders. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(3), 761–766. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00363.x
  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47(2), 149–180.
  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Cognitive correlates of developmental surface dyslexia: A single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13(1), 25–50. doi: 10.1080/026432996382051
  • Castles, A., Davis, C., Cavalot, P., & Forster, K. (2007). Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: Masked priming effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(3), 165–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.01.006
  • Coltheart, M. (2006). Dual route and connectionist models of reading: An overview. London Review of Education, 4(1), 5–17. doi: 10.1080/13603110600574322
  • Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (2000). Early morphological effects in word recognition in Hebrew: Evidence from parafoveal preview benefit. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15, 487–506. doi: 10.1080/01690960050119670
  • Di Filippo, G., De Luca, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Lexicality and stimulus length effects in Italian dyslexics: Role of the over additivity effect. Child Neuropsychology, 12(2), 141–149. doi: 10.1080/09297040500346571
  • Dominguez, A. J., Segui, J., & Cuetos, F. (2002). The time-course of inflexional morphological priming. Linguistics, 40(2), 235–260.
  • Ferreira de Oliveira, B., & dos Reis Justi, F. (2017). Morphological priming development in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 30, 1–8.
  • Friedmann, N., Dotan, D., & Rahamim, E. (2010). Is the visual analyser orthographic-specific? Reading words and numbers in letter position dyslexia. Cortex, 46(8), 982–1004. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.007
  • Frost, R. (2009). Reading in Hebrew vs. reading in English: Is there a qualitative difference? In K. Pugh & P. McCradle (Eds.), How children learn to read: Current issues and new directions in the integration of cognition, neurobiology and genetics of reading and dyslexia research and practice. New York:Psychology Press.
  • Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(5), 263–329. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11001841
  • Frost, R., Forster, K. I., & Deutsch, A. (1997). What can we learn from the morphology of Hebrew? A masked priming investigation of morphological representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 23, 829–856. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.23.4.829
  • Frost, R., Kugler, T., Deutsch, A., & Forster, K. I. (2001). Orthographic structure versus morphological structure: Principles of lexical organization in a given language. Paper presented at the Perspectives on morphological processing, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
  • Frost, R., Kugler, T., Deutsch, A., & Forster, K. I. (2005). Orthographic structure versus morphological structure: Principles of lexical organization in a given language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(6), 1293–1326. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1293
  • Giraudo, H. (2001). Rôle et représentation de l’information morphologique chez l’apprenti lecteur et l’enfant dyslexique [Role and representation of morphological information in the beginning reader and dyslexic child]. Annales du Fondation Fyssen, 16, 81–90.
  • Grainger, J., Bouttevin, S., Truc, C., Bastien, M., & Ziegler, J. (2003). Word superiority, pseudoword superiority, and learning to read: A comparison of dyslexic and normal readers. Brain & Language, 87, 432–440. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00145-7
  • Grainger, J., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2003). Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception. In P. Bonin (Ed.), Mental lexicon: Some words to talk about words (pp. 1–23). New York, NY: Nova Science.
  • Habib, M., & Giraud, K. (2013). Dyslexia. In O. Dulac, M. Lassonde, & H. B. Sarnat (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology, Vol. 111, pediatric neurology (pp. 229-236). Amsterdam: Elsevier. http://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-52891-9.00023-3
  • Kinoshita, S., Norris, D., & Siegelman, N. (2012). Transposed-letter priming effect in Hebrew in the same–different task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(7), 1296–1305. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.655749
  • Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2007). Paradoxical enhancement of letter recognition in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31(1), 61–77. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn3101_4
  • Law, J. M., Veispak, A., Vanderauwera, J., & Ghesquière, P. (2017). Morphological awareness and visual processing of derivational morphology in high-functioning adults with dyslexia: An avenue to compensation? Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(3), 483–506. doi: 10.1017/S0142716417000467
  • Layes, S., Lalonde, R., & Rebaï, M. (2014). Reading speed and phonological awareness deficits among Arabic-speaking children with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 21(1), 80–95. doi: 10.1002/dys.1491
  • Layes, S., Lalonde, R., & Rebaï, M. (2015a). Effectiveness of a phonological awareness training for arabic disabled reading children: Insights on metalinguistic benefits. Bellaterra Journal of Teaching & Learning Language & Literature, 8(4), 24–42.
  • Layes, S., Lalonde, R., Mecheri, S. & Rebaï, M. (2015b). Phonological and cognitive reading related skills as predictors of word reading and reading comprehension among Arabic dyslexic children. Psychology, 6, 20–38. http://dx.doi.org/doi.org/10.4236/psych.2015.61003
  • Lee, C. H., & Taft, M. (2009). Are onsets and codas important in processing letter position? A comparison of TL effects in English and Korean. Journal of Memory & Language, 60, 530–542. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.01.002
  • Marinelli, C. V., Angelelli, P., Di Filippo, G., & Zoccolotti, P. (2011). Is developmental dyslexia modality specific? A visual-auditory comparison of Italian dyslexics. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1718–1729. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.050
  • Marinus, E., & de Jong, P. F. (2010). Variability in word reading performance of dyslexic readers: Effects of letter length, phoneme length and digraph presence. Cortex, 46(10), 1259–1271. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.005
  • Martens, V. E. G., & de Jong, P. F. (2006). The effect of visual word features on the acquisition of orthographic knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(4), 337–356. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.11.003
  • Marslen-Wilson, W.D., Bozik, M., & Randall, B. (2008). Early decomposition in visual word recognition: Dissociation morphology, form and meaning. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(3), 394–421.
  • Oliveira, B. S. F., & Justi, F. R. R. (2017). Morphological priming development in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 30:2-8. doi: 10.1186/s41155-017-0058-8
  • Perea, M., Abu Mallouh, R., & Carreiras, M. (2010). The search of an input coding scheme: Transposed-letter priming in Arabic. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 375–380. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.3.375
  • Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J. (2003). Transposed-letter confusability effects in masked form priming. In S. Kinoshita and S. J. Lupker (Eds.), Masked priming: State of the art (pp. 97–120). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J. (2004). Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 231–246. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.005
  • Perea, M., & Perez, E. (2009). Beyond alphabetic orthographies: The role of form and phonology in transposition effects in Katakana. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(1), 67–88. doi: 10.1080/01690960802053924
  • Pitchford, N. J., Ledgeway, T., & Masterson, J. (2008). Effect of orthographic processes on letter position encoding. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(1), 97–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00363.x
  • Quémart, P., Casalis, S., & Colé, P. (2011). The role of form and meaning in the processing of written morphology: A priming study in French developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(4), 478–496. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.008
  • Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (2000). Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 507–537.
  • Schiff, R., & Raveh, M. (2007). Deficient morphological processing in adult with developmental dyslexia: Another barrier to deficient word recognition? Dyslexia, 13(2), 110–129. doi: 10.1002/dys.322
  • Schoonbaert, S., & Grainger, J. (2004). Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(3), 333–367. doi: 10.1080/01690960344000198
  • Seymour, P. H. K. (1997). Foundations of orthographic development. In C. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell (pp. 319–337). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Shalhoub-Awwad, Y., & Leikin, M. (2016). The lexical status of the root in processing morphologically complex words in Arabic. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(4), 296–310. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2016.1180525
  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2
  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading and Research Quarterly, 24(4), 402–429. doi: 10.2307/747605
  • Taha, H., & Azaizah-Seh, H. (2017). Visual word recognition and vowelization in Arabic: New evidence from lexical decision task performances. Cognitive Processing, 18(4), 521–527. doi: 10.1007/s10339-017-0830-9
  • Temple, C. M. (1997). Developmental cognitive neuropsychology. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Tsesmeli, S. N., & Seymour, P. H. K. (2009). The effects of training of morphological structure on spelling derived words by dyslexic adolescents. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 565–592. doi: 10.1348/000712608X371915
  • Van den Boer, M., De jong, P. F., & Meeteren, M. M. (2012). Lexical decision in children: Sublexical processing or lexical search? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(6), 1214–28.
  • Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2007). Cambridge University versus Hebrew University: The impact of letter transposition on reading English and Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 913–918.
  • Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2009). Letter-transposition effects are not universal: The impact of transposing letters in Hebrew. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(3), 285–302. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.05.003
  • Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. A. (2011). Morphological processing in reading acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(3), 457–466. doi: 10.1017/S0142716411000154
  • Vidyasagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (2010). Dyslexia: A deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.003
  • Wimmer, H., Schurz, M., Sturm, D., Richlan, F., Klackl, J., Kronbichler, M., & Ladurner, G. (2010). A dual-route perspective on poor reading in a regular orthography: An fMRI study. Cortex, 46(10), 1284–1298.
  • Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Lété, B., & Grainger, J. (2014). Orthographic and phonological xontributions to reading development: Tracking developmental trajectories using masked priming. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1026–1036. doi: 10.1037/a0035187

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.