38
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Contextual, Cognitive and Emotional Influences on Risk Perception for Illness

Pages 39-51 | Published online: 21 Nov 2012

Reference

  • Aaker , J. and Lee , A. 2004 . Bringing the frame into focus: The influence of regulatory fit on processing fluency and persuasion . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 86 : 205 – 218 .
  • Arkes , H.R. , Christensen , C.L. and Blumer , C. 1987 . Two methods of reducing overconfidence . Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes , 39 : 133 – 144 .
  • Armour , D.A. and Taylor , S.E. 2002 . “ When predictions fail: The dilemma of unrealistic optimism ” . In Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment , Edited by: Gilovich , T. , Griffin , D. and Kahneman , D. 334 – 347 . Cambridge : Cambridge University Press .
  • Benthin , A. , Slovic , P. , Moran , P. , Severson , H. , Mertz , C.K. and Gerrard , M. 1995 . Adolescent health-threatening and health-enhancing behaviors: A study of word association and imagery . Journal of Adolescent Health , 17 : 143 – 152 .
  • Bobbio , M. , Demichelis , B. and Guistetto , G. 1994 . Completeness of reporting trial results: Effect on physicians’ willingness to prescribe . Lancet , 343 : 1209 – 1212 .
  • Bogardus , S.T. Jr , Holmboe , E. and Jekel , J.F. 1999 . Perils, pitfalls, and possibilities in talking about medical risk . Journal of the American Medical Association , 281 : 1037 – 1041 .
  • Brase , G. L. 2002 . Which statistical formats facilitate what decisions? The perception and influence of different statistical information formats . Journal of Behavioral Decision Making , 15 : 381 – 401 .
  • Brewer , N.T. , Cuite , C.L. , Herrington , J.E. and Weinstein , N.D. 2004 . Risk perceptions and their relation to risk behavior . Annals of Behavioral Medicine , 27 : 125 – 131 .
  • Brun , W. and Tiegen , K.H. 1988 . Verbal probabilities: Ambiguous, context-dependent, or both? . Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes , 41 : 390 – 404 .
  • Bryant , G.D. and Norman , G.R. 1980 . Expressions of probability: Words and numbers . New England Journal of Medicine , 302 : 411
  • Bucher , H.C. , Weinbacher , M. and Gyr , K. 1994 . Influence of method of reporting study results on decision of physicians to prescribe drugs to lower cholesterol concentration . British Medical Journal , 309 : 761 – 764 .
  • Cameron , L.D. 2001 . “ Anxiety, cognition, and responses to health threats ” . In The self-regulation of health and illness behaviour , Edited by: Cameron , L. and Leventhal , H. 157 – 183 . London : Routledge .
  • Chaiken , S. and Trope , Y. 1999 . Dual process theories in social psychology , New York : Guilford Press .
  • Chapman , G.B. 2004 . “ The psychology of medical decision making ” . In Blackwell handbook of judgement and decision making , Edited by: Koehler , D.J. and Harvey , N. 585 – 603 . Oxford : Blackwell .
  • Christensen , C. , Heckerling , P. , Mackesy , M.E. , Bernstein , L.M. and Elstein , A.S. 1995 . Pervasiveness of framing effects in clinical decision making . Journal of Behavioral Decision Making , 8 : 169 – 180 .
  • Christensen-Szalanski , J.J. and Bushyhead , J.B. 1981 . Physicians’ use of probabilistic information in a real clinical setting . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 7 : 928 – 935 .
  • Combs , B. and Slovic , P. 1979 . Newspaper coverage of causes of death . Journalism Quarterly , 56 : 832 – 849 .
  • Connolly , T. and Reb , J. 2003 . Omission bias in vaccination decisions: Where’s the ‘omission’? Where’s the ‘bias’? . Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes , 91 : 186 – 202 .
  • Dawes , R.M. 2001 . Everyday irrationality: How pseudoscientists, lunatics, and the rest of us fail to think rationally , Oxford : Westview Press .
  • Ditto , P.H. and Croyle , R.T. 1995 . “ Understanding the impact of risk factor test results: Insights from a basic research program ” . In Psychosocial effects of screening for disease prevention and detection , Edited by: Croyle , R. T. 144 – 181 . Oxford : Oxford University Press .
  • Eddy , D.M. 1990 . The challenge . Journal of the American Medical Association , 263 : 287 – 290 .
  • Edwards , A. , Elwyn , G. and Mulley , A. 2002 . Explaining risks: Turning numerical data into meaningful pictures . British Medical Journal , 324 : 827 – 830 .
  • Facione , N.C. and Facione , P.A. 2001 . Analyzing explanations for seemingly irrational choices: Linking argument analysis and cognitive science . International Journal of Applied Philosophy , 15 : 267 – 286 .
  • Fahey , T. , Griffin , S. and Peters , T.J. 1995 . Evidence based purchasing: Understanding results of clinical trials and systematic reviews . British Medical Journal , 311 : 1056 – 1059 .
  • Finucane , M.L. , Alhakami , A. , Slovic , P. and Johnson , S.M. 2000 . The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits . Journal of Behavioral Decision Making , 13 : 1 – 17 .
  • Fischoff , B. 1995 . Risk perception and communication unplugged: Twenty years of process . Risk Analysis , 15 : 137 – 145 .
  • Fischoff , B. , Slovic , P. and Lichtenstein , S. 1977 . Knowing with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 3 : 552 – 564 .
  • Forgas , J.P. 2000 . Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition , Cambridge : Cambridge University Press .
  • Gigerenzer , G. 2002 . Reckoning with risk , London : Penguin .
  • Gilovich , T. , Griffin , D. and Kahneman , D. 2002 . Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment , Cambridge : Cambridge University Press .
  • Griffin , D.W. , Dunning , D. and Ross , L. 1990 . The role of construal processes in overconfident predictions about the self and others . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 59 : 1128 – 1139 .
  • Grimes , D.A. and Snively , G.R. 1999 . Patients’ understanding of medical risks: Implications for genetic counseling . Obstetrics & Gynecology , 93 : 910 – 914 .
  • Gurm , H. S. and Litaker , D. G. 2000 . Framing procedural risks to patients: Is 99% safe the same as a risk of 1 in 100? . Academic Medicine , 75 : 840 – 842 .
  • Halpern , D. F. , Blackman , S. and Salzman , B. 1989 . Using statistical risk information to assess oral-contraceptive safety . Applied Cognitive Psychology , 3 : 251 – 260 .
  • Hastie , R. and Dawes , R. M . 2001 . Rational choice in an uncertain world , London : Sage .
  • Hux , J.E. and Naylor , C.D. 1995 . Communicating the benefits of chronic preventative therapy: Does the format of efficacy data determine patients’ acceptance of treatment? . Medical Decision Making: An International Journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making , 15 : 152 – 157 .
  • Johnson , B.B. and Slovic , P. 1995 . Presenting uncertainty in health risk assessment: Initial studies of its effects on risk perception and trust . Risk Analysis , 15 : 485 – 494 .
  • Johnson , E. J. and Tversky , A. 1983 . Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 45 : 20 – 31 .
  • Kahneman , D. and Tversky , A. 1979 . Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk . Econometrica , 47 : 263 – 291 .
  • Kahneman , D. and Frederick , S. 2002 . “ Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment ” . In Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment , Edited by: Gilovich , T. , Griffin , D. and Kahneman , D. 49 – 81 . New York : Cambridge University Press .
  • Kahneman , D. , Slovic , P. and Tverksy , A. 1982 . Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases , Cambridge : Cambridge University Press .
  • Kalet , A. , Roberts , J. C. and Fletcher , R. H. 1994 . How do physicians talk to their patients about risk? . Journal of General Internal Medicine , 9 : 402 – 403 .
  • Keller , P.A. , Lipkus , I.M. and Rimer , B.K. 2002 . Depressive realism and health risk accuracy: The negative consequences of positive mood . Journal of Consumer Research , 29 : 57 – 69 .
  • Leventhal , H. , Nerenz , D.R. and Steele , D.J. 1984 . “ Illness representations and coping with health threats ” . In Handbook of psychology and health, volume 4: Social psychological aspects of health , Edited by: Baum , A. , Taylor , S.E. and Singer , J.E. 219 – 252 . Hillsdale , , New Jersey : Erlbaum .
  • Littenberg , B. and Nease , R.F. Jr. 1994 . When numbers get serious . Journal of General Internal Medicine , 9 : 295 – 296 .
  • Lowenstein , G.F. , Weber , E.U. , Hsee , C.K. and Welch , E.S. 2001 . Risk as feelings . Psychological Bulletin , 127 : 267 – 286 .
  • Malenka , D.J. , Baron , J.A. , Johansen , S. , Wahrenberger , J.W. and Ross , J.M. 1993 . The framing effect of relative and absolute risk . Journal of General Internal Medicine , 8 : 543 – 548 .
  • Mazur , D.J. and Hickam , D.H. 1993 . Patient interpretations of terms connoting low probabilities when communicating surgical risk . The oretical Surgery , 8 : 143 – 145 .
  • McGlone , M.S. and Reed , A.B. 1998 . Anchoring in the interpretation of probability expressions . Journal of Pragmatics , 30 : 723 – 733 .
  • McNeil , B.J. , Pauker , S.G. , Sox , H.C. and Tversky , A. 1982 . On the elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies . New England Journal of Medicine , 306 : 1259 – 1262 .
  • Nakao , M.A. and Axelrod , S. 1983 . Numbers are better than words: Verbal specifications of frequency have no place in medicine . American Journal of Medicine , 74 : 1061 – 1065 .
  • Nisbett , R. E. and Ross , L. 1980 . Human inference: strategies and shortcomings of social judgment , Englewood Cliffs , New Jersey : Prentice-Hall .
  • O’Connor , A.M. , Pennie , R.A. and Dales , R.E. 1996 . Framing effects on expectations, decisions and side effects experienced: The case of influenza immunization . Journal of Clinical Epidemiology , 49 : 1271 – 1276 .
  • O’Connor , A.M. 1989 . Effects of framing and level of probability on patients’ preferences for cancer chemotherapy . Journal of Clinical Epidemiology , 42 : 119 – 126 .
  • Paulos , J.A. 1988 . Innumeracy: Mathematical illiteracy and its consequences , London : Penguin .
  • Plous , S. 1989 . Thinking the unthinkable: The effect of anchoring on likelihood estimates of nuclear war . Journal of Applied Social Psychology , 19 : 67 – 91 .
  • Ritov , I. and Baron , J. 1990 . Reluctance to vaccinate: Omission bias and ambiguity . Journal of Behavioural Decision Making , 3 : 263 – 277 .
  • Rogers , R.W. 1983 . “ Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation ” . In Social psychology: A source book , Edited by: Cacioppo , J.R. and Petty , R.E. 153 – 176 . New York : Guilford Press .
  • Rosenstock , I.M. , Strecher , V.J. and Becker , H.M. 1988 . Social learning theory and the health belief model . Health Education Quarterly , 15 : 175 – 183 .
  • Rothman , A.J. , Kelly , K.M. , Hertel , A.W. and Salovey , P. 2003 . “ Message frames and illness representations: Implications for interventions to promote and sustain health ” . In The self-regulation of health and illness behaviour , Edited by: Cameron , L. and Leventhal , H. 278 – 196 . London : Routledge .
  • Schwarz , N. and Clore , G.L. 1983 . Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well–being: Informative and directive functions of affective states . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 45 : 513 – 523 .
  • Schwarz , N. and Clore , G. L. 2003 . Mood as information: 20 years later . Psychological Inquiry , 14 : 296 – 303 .
  • Schwarzer , R. 2001 . Social-cognitive factors in changing health-related behavior . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 10 : 47 – 51 .
  • Sheeran , P. and Abraham , C. 1996 . “ The health belief model ” . In Predicting health behaviour: Research and practice with social cognition models , Edited by: Connor , M. and Norman , P. 23 – 61 . Buckingham , UK : Open University Press .
  • Sherman , S.J. , Cialdini , R.B. , Schwartzman , D.F. and Reynolds , K.D. 1985 . Imagining can heighten or lower the perceived likelihood of contracting a disease: The mediating effect of ease of memory . Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 11 : 118 – 127 .
  • Skolbekken , J. A. 1998 . Communicating the risk reduction achieved by cholesterol reducing drugs . British Medical Journal , 316 : 1956 – 1958 .
  • Sloman , S. A. 1996 . The empirical case for two systems of reasoning . Psychological Bulletin , 119 : 3 – 22 .
  • Slovic , P. 1987 . Perception of risk . Science , 236 : 280 – 285 .
  • Slovic , P. 2000 . The perception of risk , London : Earthscan Publications .
  • Slovic , P. , Finucane , M.L. , Peters , E. and MacGregor , D.G. 2004 . Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality . Risk Analysis: An International Journal , 24 : 311 – 322 .
  • Slovic , P. , Fischoff , B. and Lichtenstein , S. 1982 . “ Facts versus fears: Understanding perceived risk ” . In Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases , Edited by: Kahneman , D. , Slovic , P. and Tversky , A. 463 – 489 . Cambridge : Cambridge University Press .
  • Slovic , P. , Monahan , J. and MacGregor , D. 2000 . Violence risk assessment and risk communication: The effects of using actual cases, providing instruction, and employing probability versus frequency formats . Law and Human Behavior , 24 : 271 – 296 .
  • Stanovich , K.E. 2002 . “ Rationality, intelligence, and levels of analysis in cognitive science: Is dysrationalia possible? ” . In Why smart people can be so stupid , Edited by: Sternberg , R. J. 124 – 158 . New Haven , Connecticut : Yale University Press .
  • Stone , E.R. , Dodrill , C.D. and Johnson , N. 2001 . Depressive cognition: A test of depressive realism versus negativity using general knowledge questions . Journal of Psychology , 135 : 583 – 603 .
  • Sunstein , C.R. 2002 . Probability neglect: Emotions, worst cases, and law . Yale Law Journal , 112 : 61 – 107 .
  • Thompson , S.C. , Armstrong , W. and Thomas , C. 1998 . Illusions of control, underestimations, and accuracy: A control heuristic explanation . Psychological Bulletin , 123 : 143 – 161 .
  • Timmermans , D. 1994 . The roles of experience and domain of expertise in using numerical and verbal probability terms in medical decisions . Medical Decisions Making , 9 : 2 – 11 .
  • Tripp , G. , Tan , S. and Milne , J . 1995 . Risk perception and anxiety . New Zealand Journal of Psychology , 24 : 37 – 43 .
  • Walker , E.A. , Kalten , M.R. , Mertz , C.K. and Flynn , J. 2003 . Risk perception for developing diabetes: Comparative risk judgments of physicians . Diabetes Care , 26 : 2543 – 2548 .
  • Wallsten , T.S. , Budescu , D.V. and Zwick , R. 1993 . Comparing the calibration and coherence of numerical and verbal probability judgments . Management Science , 39 : 176 – 190 .
  • Weinstein , N.D. 1998 . Accuracy of smokers’ risk perceptions . Annals of Behavioral Medicine , 20 : 135 – 140 .
  • Weinstein , N.D. 1999 . What does it mean to understand a risk? Evaluating risk comprehension . Journal of the National Cancer Institute, Monograph , 25 : 15 – 20 .
  • Weinstein , N.D. , Atwood , K. and Puleo , E. 2004 . Colon cancer: Risk perceptions and risk communication . Journal of Health Communication , 9 : 53 – 65 .
  • Weinstein , N.D. and Klein , W.M. 1995 . Resistance of personal risk perceptions to debiasing interventions . Health Psychology , 14 : 132 – 140 .
  • Wiebe , D.J. and Korbel , C. 2001 . “ Defensive denial, affect, and the self-regulation of health threats ” . In The self-regulation of health and illness behaviour , Edited by: Cameron , L. and Leventhal , H. 184 – 203 . London : Routledge .
  • Wilson , D.K. , Kaplan , R.M. and Schneiderman , L.J. 1987 . Framing of decisions and selections of alternatives in health care . Social Behaviour , 2 : 51 – 59 .
  • Windschitl , P. D. and Wells , G. L. 1996 . Measuring psychological uncertainty: Verbal versus numeric methods . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied , 2 : 343 – 364 .
  • Woloshin , K.K. , Ruffin , M.T. and Gorenflo , D.W. 1994 . Patients’ interpretation of qualitative probability statements . Archives of Family Medicine , 3 : 961 – 966 .
  • Wright , W. and Bower , G.H. 1992 . Mood effects on subjective probability assessment . Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes , 52 : 276 – 291 .
  • Wu , W. C. and Pearlman , R. A. 1988 . Consent in medical decision making: The role of communication . Journal of General Internal Medicine , 3 : 9 – 14 .
  • Yamagishi , K. 1997 . When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: Implications for risk communication . Applied Cognitive Psychology , 11 : 495 – 506 .

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.