5,470
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empathy and Understanding Others

The practical other: teleology and its development

ORCID Icon, &

References

  • Anscombe, G. E. 1957. Intention. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Apperly, I. A., and S. A. Butterfill. 2009. “Do Humans Have Two Systems to Track Beliefs and Belief-Like States?” Psychological Review 116: 953–970. doi: 10.1037/a0016923
  • Barlassina, Luca, and Robert M. Gordon. 2017. “Folk Psychology as Mental Simulation.” In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2017 Edition), edited by Edward N. Zalta. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2017/entries/folkpsych-simulation.
  • Brand, M. 1984. Intending and Acting. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Bratman, M. E. 2009. “Intention, Practical Rationality, and Self-Governance.” Ethics 119 (3): 411–443. doi: 10.1086/599985
  • Breithaupt, F. 2018. “The Bad Things We Do because of Empathy.” Interdisciplinary Science Reviews 43 (2). doi:10.1080/03080188.2018.1450928.
  • Buddeberg, E. 2018. “Thinking the Other, Thinking Otherwise: Levinas’ Conception of Responsibility.” Interdisciplinary Science Reviews 43 (2). doi:10.1080/03080188.2018.1450927.
  • Carruthers, P. 2011. The Opacity of Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Charles, D. 2012. “Teleological Causation.” In The Oxford Handbook of Aristotle, edited by C. Shields, 227–266. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Churchland, P. M. 1984. Matter and Consciousness: A Contemporary Introduction to the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Clark, E. V. 1997. “Conceptual Perspective and Lexical Choice in Acquisition.” Cognition 64: 1–37. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00010-3.
  • Csibra, G., and G. Gergely. 1998. “The Teleological Origins of Mentalistic Action Explanations: A Developmental Hypothesis.” Developmental Science 1: 255–259. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00039.
  • Davidson, D. 1963. “Actions, Reasons, and Causes.” Journal of Philosophy 60: 685–700. doi:10.2307/2023177.
  • Engelen, E.-M. 2018. “Can We Share an Us-Feeling with a Digital Machine? Emotional Sharing and the Recognition of One as Another.” Interdisciplinary Science Reviews 43 (2). doi:10.1080/03080188.2018.1450814.
  • Evans, G. 1982. The Varieties of Reference. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gallagher, S. 2007. “Simulation Trouble.” Social Neuroscience 2 (3-4): 353–365. doi: 10.1080/17470910601183549
  • Goldman, A. I. 2006. Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gollek, C., and M. J. Doherty. 2016. Metacognitive Developments in Word Learning: Mutual Exclusivity and Theory of Mind.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 148: 51–69. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.007.
  • Gopnik, A., and A. N. Meltzoff. 1997. Word, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Gordon, R. M. 1995. “Simulation Without Introspection or Inference from Me to You.” In Mental Simulation: Evaluations and Applications, edited by M. Davies and T. Stone, 53–67. Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Gordon, R. M. 2007. “Ascent Routines for Propositional Attitudes.” Synthese 159: 151–165. doi:10.1007/s11229-007-9202-9.
  • Haryu, E., and M. Imai. 1999. “Controlling the Application of Mutual Exclusivity Assumption in the Acquisition of Lexical Hierarchies.” Japanese Psychological Research 41 (1): 21–34. doi: 10.1111/1468-5884.00102
  • Hornsby, J. 2008. “A Disjunctive Conception of Acting for Reasons.” In Disjunctivism: Perception, Action, Knowledge, edited by A. Haddock and F. Macpherson, 244–261. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Kanske, P. 2018. “The Social Mind: Disentangling Affective and Cognitive Routes to Understanding Others.” Interdisciplinary Science Reviews 43 (2). doi:10.1080/03080188.2018.1453243.
  • Leahy, B., E. Rafetseder, and J. Perner. 2017. “Teleology-in-Perspective: The Role of Counterfactual Reasoning in False Belief Reasoning.” Poster presented at the Second Biennial International Convention of Psychological Science (ICPS), Vienna.
  • Musholt, K. (2018). “Self and Others.” Interdisciplinary Science Reviews 43 (2). doi:10.1080/03080188.2018.1453245.
  • Perner, J., and F. Esken. 2015. “Evolution of Human Cooperation in Homo heidelbergensis: Teleology Versus Mentalism.” Developmental Review 38: 69–88. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.005.
  • Perner, J., and J. Roessler. 2010. “Teleology and Causal Reasoning in Children’s Theory of Mind.” In Causing Human Action: New Perspectives on the Causal Theory of Action, edited by J. Aguilar and A. Buckareff, 199–228. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Perner, J., and J. Roessler. 2012. “From Infants’ to Children’s Appreciation of Belief.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (10): 519–525. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.004.
  • Premack, D., and G. Woodruff. 1978. “Does the Chimpanzee Have a Theory of Mind?” The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1: 516–526.
  • Priewasser, B., J. Roessler, and J. Perner. 2013. “Competition as Rational Action: Why Young Children Cannot Appreciate Competitive Games.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 116 (2): 545–559. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.008.
  • Rafetseder, E., R. Cristi-Vargas, and J. Perner. 2010. “Counterfactual Reasoning: Developing a Sense of ‘Nearest Possible World’.” Child Development 81 (1): 376–389. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01401.x
  • Rafetseder, E., M. Schwitalla, and J. Perner. 2013. “Counterfactual Reasoning: From Childhood to Adulthood.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 114 (3): 389–404. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.010.
  • Raz, J. 1999. “Explaining Normativity: On Rationality and the Justification of Reason.” Ratio 12: 354–379.
  • Riggs, K. J., D. M. Peterson, E. J. Robinson, and P. Mitchell. 1998. “Are Errors in False Belief Tasks Symptomatic of a Broader Difficulty with Counterfactuality?” Cognitive Development 13 (1): 73–90. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90021-1
  • Roessler, J. 2014. “Reason Explanation and the Second-Person Perspective.” Philosophical Explorations 17 (3): 346–357. doi: 10.1080/13869795.2014.946954
  • Roessler, J., and J. Perner. 2013. “Teleology: Belief as Perspective.” In UOM-3: Understanding Other Minds, edited by S. Baron-Cohen, M. Lombardo, and H. Tager-Flusberg, 3rd ed., 35–50. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Roessler, J., and J. Perner. 2015. “Pro-Social Cognition: Helping, Practical Reasons, and ‘Theory of Mind’.” Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 14 (4): 755–767. doi:10.1007/s11097-015-9438-7.
  • Scanlon, T. 1998. What We Owe to Each Other. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Sellars, W. 1956. “Empiricism and the Philosophy of Mind.” In Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, edited by K. Gunderson, 253–329. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
  • Tomasello, M. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Wellman, H. M. 1990. The Child’s Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Wellman, H. M., D. Cross, and J. Watson. 2001. “Meta-analysis of Theory of Mind Development: The Truth About False Belief.” Child Development 72: 655–684. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304.
  • Wimmer, H., and J. Perner. 1983. “Beliefs About Beliefs: Representation and Constraining Function of Wrong Beliefs in Young Children’s Understanding of Deception.” Cognition 13: 103–128. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5.