Publication Cover
Experimental Aging Research
An International Journal Devoted to the Scientific Study of the Aging Process
Volume 46, 2020 - Issue 5
4,531
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Cognitive and Affective Theory of Mind in Healthy Aging

, &
Pages 382-395 | Received 27 Aug 2019, Accepted 27 Jul 2020, Published online: 05 Aug 2020

References

  • Abu-Akel, A., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2011). Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2971–2984.
  • Adjeroud, N., Besnard, J., Massioui, N. E., Verny, C., Prudean, A., Scherer, C., … Allain, P. (2016). Theory of mind and empathy in preclinical and clinical Huntington’s disease. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 89–99.
  • Baksh, R. A., Abrahams, S., Auyeung, B., & MacPherson, S. E. (2018). The Edinburgh Social Cognition Test (ESCoT): Examining the effects of age on a new measure of theory of mind and social norm understanding. PLoS ONE, 13, e0195818.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175.
  • Bottiroli, S., Cavallini, E., Ceccato, I., Vecchi, T., & Lecce, S. (2016). Theory of mind in aging: Comparing cognitive and affective components in the faux pas test. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 62, 152–162.
  • Cavallini, E., Bianco, F., Bottiroli, S., Rosi, A., Vecchi, T., & Lecce, S. (2015). Training for generalization in Theory of Mind: a study with older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 1123.
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed. ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42–45.
  • DeCoster, J. (2012, June 19). Converting effect sizes. Retrieved from http://www.stat-help.com
  • Duval, C., Piolino, P., Bejanin, A., Eustache, F., & Desgranges, B. (2011). Age effects on different components of theory of mind. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 627–642.
  • Fischer, A. L., O’Rourke, N., & Loken Thornton, W. (2017). Age differences in cognitive and affective theory of mind: Concurrent contributions of neurocognitive performance, sex, and pulse pressure. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 72, 71–81.
  • Frank, C. K., Baron-Cohen, S., & Ganzel, B. L. (2015). Sex differences in the neural basis of false-belief and pragmatic language comprehension. NeuroImage, 105, 300–311.
  • Hall, J. A., & Matsumoto, D. (2004). Gender differences in judgments of multiple emotions from facial expressions. Emotion, 4, 201–206.
  • Happé, F. G., Winner, E., & Brownell, H. (1998). The getting of wisdom: Theory of mind in old age. Developmental Psychology, 34, 358–3362.
  • Healy, M. L., & Grossman, M. (2018). Cognitive and affective perspective-taking: Evidence for shared and dissociable anatomical substrates. Frontiers in Neurology, 9, 491.
  • Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Ruffman, T., & Bailey, P. E. (2013). A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychology and Aging, 28, 826–839.
  • IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 25.0). [Computer software].
  • JASP Team. (2020). JASP (Version 0.11.1). [Computer software].
  • Kalbe, E., Schlegel, M., Sack, A. T., Nowak, D. A., Dafotakis, M., Bangard, C., … Kessler, J. (2010). Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: A TMS study. Cortex, 46, 769–780.
  • Kemp, J., Després, O., Sellal, F., & Dufour, A. (2012). Theory of Mind in normal ageing and neurodegenerative pathologies. Ageing Research Reviews, 11, 199–219.
  • Kim, E., Kyeong, S., Cheon, K. A., Park, B., Oh, M. K., Chun, J. W., … Song, D. H. (2016). Neural responses to affective and cognitive theory of mind in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 621, 117–125.
  • Krause, L., Enticott, P. G., Zangen, A., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2012). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in theory of mind: A deep rTMS study. Behavioural Brain Research, 228, 87–90.
  • Lackner, C., Sabbagh, M. A., Hallinan, E., Liu, X., & Holden, J. J. (2012). Dopamine receptor D4 gene variation predicts preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. Developmental Science, 15, 272–280.
  • Lecce, S., Ceccato, I., Rosi, A., Bianco, F., Bottiroli, S., & Cavallini, E. (2019). Theory of mind plasticity in aging: The role of baseline, verbal knowledge, and executive functions. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 29, 440–455.
  • Lee, I. A., & Preacher, K. J. (2013). Calculation for the test of the difference between two dependent correlations with one variable in common [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://quantpsy.org
  • Li, D., Li, X., Yu, F., Chen, X., Zhang, L., Li, D., … Wang, K. (2017). Comparing the ability of cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescent onset schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 13, 937.
  • Li, X., Wang, K., Wang, F., Tao, Q., Xie, Y., & Cheng, Q. (2013). Aging of theory of mind: The influence of educational level and cognitive processing. International Journal of Psychology, 48, 715–727.
  • Moran, J. M. (2013). Lifespan development: The effects of typical aging on theory of mind. Behavioural Brain Research, 237, 32–40.
  • Moran, J. M., Jolly, E., & Mitchell, J. P. (2012). Social-cognitive deficits in normal aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 5553–5561.
  • Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4, 61–64.
  • Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., ... & Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 695–699.
  • Pezzuti, L., Longobardi, E., Milletti, K., & Ovidi, A. (2011). A study about the Theory of Mind in primary and secondary aging. Life Span and Disability, 14, 31–44.
  • Poletti, M., Enrici, I., & Adenzato, M. (2012). Cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in neurodegenerative diseases: Neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 2147–2164.
  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.
  • Rakoczy, H., Harder-Kasten, A., & Sturm, L. (2012). The decline of theory of mind in old age is (partly) mediated by developmental changes in domain-general abilities. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 58–72.
  • Rauers, A., Blanke, E., & Riediger, M. (2013). Everyday empathic accuracy in younger and older couples: Do you need to see your partner to know his or her feelings? Psychological Science, 24, 2210–2217.
  • Richter, D., & Kunzmann, U. (2011). Age differences in three facets of empathy: Performance-based evidence. Psychology and Aging, 26, 60–70.
  • Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.
  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2008). Recognition of ‘fortune of others’ emotions in Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1451–1461.
  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3054–3067.
  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Shur, S., Barcai-Goodman, L., Medlovich, S., Harari, H., & Levkovitz, Y. (2007). Dissociation of cognitive from affective components of theory of mind in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 149, 11–23.
  • Slessor, G., Phillips, L. H., & Bull, R. (2007). Exploring the specificity of age-related differences in theory of mind tasks. Psychology and Aging, 22, 639.
  • Turkstra, L. S., Norman, R. S., Mutlu, B., & Duff, M. C. (2018). Impaired theory of mind in adults with traumatic brain injury: A replication and extension of findings. Neuropsychologia, 111, 117–122.
  • Walzak, L. C., & Loken Thornton, W. (2018). The role of illness burden in Theory of Mind performance among older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 44, 427–442.
  • Wang, Z., & Su, Y. (2013). Age-related differences in the performance of theory of mind in older adults: A dissociation of cognitive and affective components. Psychology and Aging, 28, 284–291.
  • Xia, H., Wu, N., & Su, Y. (2012). Investigating the genetic basis of Theory of Mind (ToM): The role of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene polymorphisms. PLoS One, 7, e49768.
  • Young, L., Dodell-Feder, D., & Saxe, R. (2010). What gets the attention of the temporo-parietal junction? An fMRI investigation of attention and theory of mind. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2658–2664.
  • Zhang, X., Lecce, S., Ceccato, I., Cavallini, E., Zhang, L., & Chen, T. (2019). Plasticity in older adults’ theory of mind performance: The impact of motivation. Aging & Mental Health, 22, 1592–1599.