Publication Cover
Experimental Aging Research
An International Journal Devoted to the Scientific Study of the Aging Process
Volume 49, 2023 - Issue 1
404
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Implicit and Explicit Age Stereotypes Assessed in the Same Contexts are Still Independent

&
Pages 41-57 | Received 25 May 2021, Accepted 25 Jan 2022, Published online: 17 Feb 2022

References

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888–918. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888
  • Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Human associative memory. Washington, DC: Winston & Sons.
  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP) and the relational elaboration and coherence (REC) model. The Psychological Record, 60(3), 527–542. doi:10.1007/BF03395726
  • Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(3), 242–261. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0603_8
  • Blair, I. V., & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 1142–1163. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1142
  • Bluemke, M., & Friese, M. (2006). Do features of stimuli influence IAT effects? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2), 163–176. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.004
  • Cameron, C. D., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., & Payne, B. K. (2012). Sequential priming measures of implicit social cognition: A meta-analysis of associations with behavior and explicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(4), 330–350. doi:10.1177/1088868312440047
  • Casper, C., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic stereotype activation is context dependent. Social Psychology, 41(3), 131–136. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000019
  • Casper, C., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2011). The activation of specific facets of age stereotypes depends on individuating information. Social Cognition, 29(4), 393–414. doi:10.1521/soco.2011.29.4.393
  • Casper, C., & Rothermund, K. (2012). Gender self-stereotyping is context-dependent for men but not for women. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 34(5), 434–442. doi:10.1080/01973533.2012.712014
  • Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., & Groom, C. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(4), 469–487. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.469
  • Cummins, J., & De Houwer, J. (2021). The shape of belief: Developing a mouse tracking-based relational implicit measure. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(8), 1517–1526. doi:10.1177/1948550620978019
  • De Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learning & Behavior, 37(1), 1–20. doi:10.3758/LB.37.1.1
  • De Houwer, J. (2014). Why a propositional single-process model of associative learning deserves to be defended. In J. W. Sherman & B. Gawronski (Eds.), Dual processes in social psychology (pp. 530–541). New York, NY: Guilford.
  • De Houwer, J., Geldof, T., & De Bruycker, E. (2005). The Implicit Association Test as a general measure of similarity. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 59(4), 228–239. doi:10.1037/h0087478
  • De Houwer, J., Heider, N., Spruyt, A., Roets, A., & Hughes, S. (2015). The relational responding task: Toward a new implicit measure of beliefs. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 319–327. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00319
  • Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5–18. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.56.1.5
  • Diekman, A. B., & Hirnisey, L. (2007). The effect of context on the silver ceiling: A role congruity perspective on prejudiced responses. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(10), 1353–1366. doi:10.1177/0146167207303019
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146
  • Fiedler, K., Messner, C., & Bluemke, M. (2006). Unresolved problems with the “I,” the “A” and the “T”: Logical and psychometric critique of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). European Review of Social Psychology, 17(1), 74–147. doi:10.1080/10463280600681248
  • Gast, A., & Rothermund, K. (2010). When old and frail is not the same: Dissociating category and stimulus effects in four implicit attitude measurement methods. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(3), 479–498. doi:10.1080/17470210903049963
  • Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2009). Operating principles versus operating conditions in the distinction between associative and propositional processes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(2), 207–208. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09000958
  • Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4
  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464
  • Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 17–41. doi:10.1037/a0015575
  • Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(10), 1369–1385. doi:10.1177/0146167205275613
  • Hummert, M. L., Garstka, T. A., O’Brien, L. T., Greenwald, A. G., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). Using the Implicit Association Test to measure age differences in implicit social cognitions. Psychology and Aging, 17(3), 482–495. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.3.482
  • Kidder, C. K., White, K. R., Hinojos, M. R., Sandoval, M., & Crites, S. L. J. (2018). Sequential stereotype priming: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(3), 199–227. doi:10.1177/1088868317723532
  • Kite, M. E., Stockdale, G. D., Whitley, B. E., & Johnson, B. T. (2005). Attitudes toward younger and older adults: An updated meta-analytic review. Journal of Social Issues, 61(2), 241–266. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00404.x
  • Kornadt, A. E., Meissner, F., & Rothermund, K. (2016). Implicit and explicit age stereotypes for specific life-domains across the life span: Distinct patterns and age group differences. Experimental Aging Research, 42(2), 195–211. doi:10.1080/0361073X.2016.1132899
  • Kornadt, A. E., & Rothermund, K. (2011). Contexts of aging: Assessing evaluative age stereotypes in different life domains. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B(5), 547–556. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr036
  • Kornadt, A. E., & Rothermund, K. (2012). Internalization of age stereotypes into the self-concept via future self-views: A general model and domain-specific differences. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 164–172. doi:10.1037/a0025110
  • Kurdi, B., Seitchik, A. E., Axt, J. R., Carroll, T. J., Karapetyan, A., Kaushik, N., & Banaji, M. R. (2019). Relationship between the implicit association test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 74(5), 569–586. doi:10.1037/amp0000364
  • McFarland, S. G., & Crouch, Z. (2002). A cognitive skill confound on the Implicit Association Test. Social Cognition, 20(6), 483–510. doi:10.1521/soco.20.6.483.22977
  • Meissner, F., Grigutsch, L. A., Koranyi, N., Müller, F., & Rothermund, K. (2019). Predicting behavior with implicit measures: Disillusioning findings, reasonable explanations, and sophisticated solutions. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2483–2498. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02483
  • Meissner, F., & Rothermund, K. (2013). Estimating the contributions of associations and recoding in the Implicit Association Test: The ReAL model for the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 45–69. doi:10.1037/a0030734
  • Meissner, F., & Rothermund, K. (2015). The insect-nonword IAT revisited: Dissociating between evaluative associations and recoding. Social Psychology, 46(1), 46–54. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000220
  • Meyer, D. E. (1970). On the representation and retrieval of stored semantic information. Cognitive Psychology, 1(3), 242–300. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(70)90017-4
  • Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1976). Meaning, memory structure, and mental processes. Science, 192(4234), 27–33. doi:10.1126/science.1257753
  • Mierke, J., & Klauer, K. C. (2003). Method-specific variance in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1180–1192. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1180
  • Minear, M., & Park, D. C. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 36, 630–633. doi:10.3758/BF03206543
  • Müller, F., & Rothermund, K. (2012). Talking loudly but lazing at work – Behavioral effects of stereotypes are context dependent. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(5), 557–563. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1869
  • Müller, F., & Rothermund, K. (2014). What does it take to activate stereotypes? Simple primes don’t seem enough. Social Psychology, 45(3), 187–193. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000183
  • Müller, F., & Rothermund, K. (2019). The propositional evaluation paradigm (PEP): Indirect assessment of personal beliefs and attitudes. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2385–2398. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02385
  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibition less spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 106, 226–254. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.106.3.226
  • Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Nelson, T. D. (Ed.). (2002). Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6, 101–115. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101
  • Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., Ranganath, K. A., … Banaji, M. R. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18(1), 36–88. doi:10.1080/10463280701489053
  • Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., & Tetlock, P. E. (2013). Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination: A meta-analysis of IAT criterion studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(2), 171–192. doi:10.1037/a0032734
  • Perry, E. L., & Finkelstein, L. M. (1999). Toward a broader view of age discrimination in employment-related decisions: A joint consideration of organizational factors and cognitive processes. Human Resource Management Review, 9(1), 21–49. doi:10.1016/S1053-4822(99)00010-8
  • Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the Implicit Association Test: Dissociating salience from associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 133(2), 139–165. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.139
  • Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Gonsalkorale, K., Hugenberg, K., Allen, T. J., & Groom, C. J. (2008). The self-regulation of automatic associations and behavioral impulses. Psychological Review, 115(2), 314–335. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.314
  • Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the Implicit Association Test immune to faking? Experimental Psychology, 51(3), 165–179. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.51.3.165
  • Steffens, M. C., & Plewe, I. (2001). Items’ cross-category associations as a confounding factor in the Implicit Association Test. Zeitschrift für experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 123–134. doi:10.1026/0949-3946.48.2.123
  • Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., & Sherman, J. W. (2010). A practical guide to implicit association tests and related tasks. In B. Gawronski, and B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition (pp. 117–139). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
  • Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Voss, P., Kornadt, A. E., Hess, T. M., Fung, H. H., & Rothermund, K. (2018). A world of difference? Domain-specific views on aging in China, the US, and Germany. Psychology and Aging, 33(4), 595–606. doi:10.1037/pag0000237
  • Wentura, D., & Degner, J. (2010). A practical guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In B. Gawronski, and B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 95–116). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
  • Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (2007). Paradigms we live by: A plea for more basic research on the Implicit Association Test. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 195–215). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (2014). Priming is not priming is not priming. Social Cognition, 32, 47–67. doi:10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.47
  • Wigboldus, D. H., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). When stereotypes get in the way: Stereotypes obstruct stereotype-inconsistent trait inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 470–484. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.84.3.470
  • Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107(1), 101–126. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.101
  • Wittenbrink, B. (2007). Measuring attitudes through priming. In B. Wittenbrink, and N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 17–58). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
  • Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(2), 262–274. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.262
  • Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (2001). Spontaneous prejudice in context: Variability in automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 815–827. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.815
  • Zaniboni, S., Kmicinska, M., Truxillo, D. M., Kahn, K., Paladino, M. P., & Fraccaroli, F. (2019). Will you still hire me when I am over 50? The effects of implicit and explicit age stereotyping on resume evaluations. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 28(4), 453–467. doi:10.1080/1359432X.2019.1600506

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.