Publication Cover
Chronobiology International
The Journal of Biological and Medical Rhythm Research
Volume 36, 2019 - Issue 12
976
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Why do evening people consider themselves more intelligent than morning individuals? The role of big five, narcissism, and objective cognitive ability

ORCID Icon, & ORCID Icon
Pages 1741-1751 | Received 30 Jul 2019, Accepted 10 Oct 2019, Published online: 23 Oct 2019

References

  • Ackerman PL, Heggestad ED. 1997. Intelligence, personality, and interests: evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin. 121(2):219–245.
  • Ackerman PL, Wolman SD. 2007. Determinants and validity of self-estimates of abilities and self-concept measures. J Exp Psychol. 13:57–78.
  • Bazińska R, Drat-Ruszczak K. 2000. The structure of narcissism measured with polish adaptation of raskin and hall NPI. Czasopismo Psychologiczne. 6:171–187.
  • Bonke J. 2012. Do morning-type people earn more than evening-type people? How chronotypes influence income. Ann Econ Stat. 105:55–72.
  • Brummelman E, Thomaes S, Sedikides C. 2016. Separating narcissism from self-esteem. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 25:8–13.
  • Campbell WK, Foster JD. 2007. The narcissistic self: background, an extended agency model, and ongoing controversies. In: Sedikides C, Spencer S, editors. Frontiers in social psychology: the self. Philadelphia (PA): Psychology Press; p. 115–138.
  • Carciofo R, Yang J, Song N, Du F, Zhang K, Qiu J. 2016. Psychometric evaluation of Chinese-language 44-item and 10-item big five personality inventories, including correlations with chronotype, mindfulness and mind wandering. PLoS One. 11(2):e0149963.
  • Cattell RB. 1973. Measuring intelligence with the culture-fair tests. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.
  • Chamorro-Premuzic T, Furnham A. 2004. A possible model for explaining the personality-intelligence interface. Br J Psychol. 95:249–264.
  • Chamorro-Premuzic T, Furnham A. 2006. Intellectual competence and the intelligent personality: a third way in differential psychology. Rev Gen Psychol. 10:251–267.
  • Chamorro-Premuzic T, Moutafi J, Furnham A. 2005. The relationship between personality traits, subjectively-assessed and fluid intelligence. Pers Individ Dif. 7:1517–1528.
  • Chamorro-Premuzic T, Quiroga MA, Colom R. 2009. Intellectual competence and academic performance: a Spanish study. Learn Individual Differences. 19(4). p. 486–491.
  • Costa PT, McCrae RR. 1992. Revised neo personality inventory (neo pi-r) and neo five-factor inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Deary IJ, Strand S, Smith P, Fernandes C. 2007. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence. 35:13–21.
  • DeYoung CG. 2015. Openness/intellect: a dimension of personality reflecting cognitive exploration. In M. Mikulincer, P. R. Shaver, M. L. Cooper, & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), APA handbooks in psychology. APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Vol. 4. Personality processes and individual differences (pp. 369–399). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association
  • Drezno M, Stolarski M, Matthews G. 2019. An in-depth look into the association between morningness–eveningness and well-being: evidence for mediating and moderating effects of personality. Chronobiol Int. 36(1):96–109.
  • Dufner M, Denissen JJA, van Zalk M, Matthes B, Meeus WHJ, van Aken MAG, Sedikides C. 2012. Positive intelligence illusions: on the relation between intellectual self-enhancement and psychological adjustment. J Pers. 80:537–572.
  • Enright T, Refinetti R. 2017. Chronotype, class times, and academic achievement of university students. Chronobiol Int. 34(4):445–450.
  • Freund P, Kasten N. 2012. How smart do you think you are? A meta-analysis on the validity of self-estimates of cognitive ability. Psychol Bull. 138:296–321.
  • Gignac GE, Stough C, Loukomitis S. 2004. Openness, intelligence, and self-report intelligence. Intelligence. 32(2):133–143.
  • Gold B, Kuhn JT. 2017. A Longitudinal Study on the stability of self-estimated intelligence and its relationship to personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences. 106:292–297.
  • Goldberg LR. 1992. The development of markers for the big-five factor structure. Psychol Assess. 4:26–42.
  • Haraszti RÁ, Ella K, Gyöngyösi N, Roenneberg T, Káldi K. 2014. Social jetlag negatively correlates with academic performance in undergraduates. Chronobiol Int. 31:603–612.
  • Hayes AF. 2015. An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 50:1–22.
  • Horne JA, Östberg O. 1976. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 4:97–110.
  • Howard MC, Cogswell J. 2018. The “other” relationships of self-assessed intelligence: a meta-analysis. J Res Pers. 77:31–46.
  • Jankowski KS. 2012. Morningness/eveningness and satisfaction with life in a polish sample. Chronobiol Int. 29:780–785.
  • Jankowski KS. 2015. Composite scale of morningness: psychometric properties, validity with munich chronotype questionnaire and age/sex differences in Poland. Eur Psychiatry. 30:166–171.
  • Jonason PK, Jones A, Lyons M. 2013. Creatures of the night: chronotypes and the dark triad traits. Pers Individ Dif. 55:538–541.
  • Jonason PK, Webster GD. 2010. The dirty dozen: a concise measure of the dark triad. Psychol Assess. 22:420–432.
  • Judge TA, Simon LS, Hurst C, Kelley K. 2014. What I experienced yesterday is who I am today: relationship of work motivations and behaviors to within individual variation in the five-factor model of personality. J Appl Psychol. 99:199.
  • Kanazawa S, Perina K. 2009. Why night owls are more intelligent. Pers Individ Dif. 47:685–690.
  • Kolomeichuk SN, Randler C, Shabalina I, Fradkova L, Borisenkov M. 2016. The influence of chronotype on the academic achievement of children and adolescents–evidence from Russian Karelia. Biol Rhythm Res. 47(6):873–883.
  • Korczak AL, Martynhak BJ, Pedrazzoli M, Brito AF, Louzada FM. 2008. Influence of chronotype and social zeitgebers on sleep/wake patterns. Braz J Med Biol Res. 41(10):914–919.
  • Lurie SJ, Meldrum S, Nofziger AC, Sillin LF III, Mooney CJ, Epstein RM. 2007. Changes in self-perceived abilities among male and female medical students after the first year of clinical training. Med Teach. 29:921–926.
  • Marvel-Coen J, Nickels N, Maestripieri D. 2018. The relationship between morningness–eveningness, psychosocial variables, and cortisol reactivity to stress from a life history perspective. Evol Behav Sci. 12(2):71.
  • Matczak A, Jaworowska A, Martowska K. 2013. Test rozumienia słów. Podrecznik [Test of words comprehension, Manual]. Warsaw: PTP.
  • May CP. 1999. Synchrony effects in cognition: the costs and a benefit. Psychon Bull Rev. 6:142–147.
  • Mayer JD, Salovey P, Caruso DR. 2008. Emotional intelligence: new ability or eclectic traits? Am Psychologist. 63(6):503–517.
  • Ş Beşoluk, Önder I, Deveci İ. 2011. Morningness–eveningness preferences and academic achievement of university students. Chronobiol Int. 28:118–125.
  • Milfont TL, Schwarzenthal M. 2014. Explaining why larks are future-oriented and owls are present-oriented: self-control mediates the chronotype–time perspective relationships. Chronobiol Int. 31(4):581–588.
  • Miller JD, Hoffman BJ, Gaughan ET, Gentile B, Maples J, Campbell WK. 2011. Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: a nomological network analysis. J Pers. 79(5):1013–1042.
  • Natale V, Cicogna PC. 2002. Morningness–eveningness dimension: is it really a continuum? Pers Individ Dif. 32:809–816.
  • Neisser U, Boodoo G, Bouchard TJ Jr., Boykin AW, Brody N, Ceci SJ, … Urbina S. 1996. Intelligence: knowns and unknowns. Am Psychologist. 51:77–101.
  • Paulhus DL, Robins RW, Trzesniewski KH, Tracy JL. 2004. Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research. Multivariate Behav Res. 39:303–328.
  • Peterson ER, Whiteman MC. 2007. “I think I can, I think I can … ”: the interrelationships among self-assessed intelligence, self-concept, self-efficacy and the personality trait intellect in university students in Scotland and New Zealand. Pers Individ Dif. 43(4):959–968.
  • Preckel F, Lipnevich AA, Schneider S, Roberts RD. 2011. Chronotype, cognitive abilities, and academic achievement: a meta-analytic investigation. Learn Individ Differ. 21:483–492.
  • Rahafar A, Randler C, Castellana I, Kausch I. 2017. How does chronotype mediate gender effect on Dark Triad? Pers Individ Dif. 108:35–39.
  • Randler C, Baumann VP, Horzum MB. 2014. Morningness–eveningness, big five and the BIS/BAS inventory. Pers Individ Dif. 66:64–67.
  • Randler C, Faßl C, Kalb N. 2017. From lark to owl: developmental changes in morningness-eveningness from new-borns to early adulthood. Sci Rep. 7:45874.
  • Raskin RN, Terry H. 1988. A principal components analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 54:890–902.
  • Roberts RD, Kyllonen PC. 1999. Morningness–eveningness and intelligence: early to bed, early to rise will likely make you anything but wise! Pers Individ Dif. 27:1123–1133.
  • Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Pramstaller PP, Ricken J, Havel M, Guth A, Merrow M. 2004. A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr Biol. 14:1038–1039.
  • Smith CS, Reilly C, Midkiff K. 1989. Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestion for an improved measure of morningness. Journal of Applied Psychology. 75. 728–38.
  • Song J, Stough C. 2000. The relationship between morningness-eveningness, time-of-day, speed of information processing, and intelligence. Pers Individ Dif. 29:1179–1190.
  • Stańczak J. 2013. Polish adaptation of cft-2-r. Warsaw: PTP.
  • Strus W, Cieciuch J, Rowiński T. 2014. Polish adaptation of IPIP-BFM-50 measuring five personality traits in a lexical approach. Roczniki Psychologiczne. 17:327–346.
  • Tonetti L, Fabbri M, Natale V. 2009. Relationship between circadian typology and big five personality domains. Chronobiol Int. 26(2):337–347.
  • Tsaousis I. 2010. Circadian preferences and personality traits: a meta-analysis. Eur J Pers. 24. 356–373.
  • Valladares M, Ramírez-Tagle R, Muñoz MA, Obregón AM. 2018. Individual differences in chronotypes associated with academic performance among Chilean University students. Chronobiol Int. 35:578–583.
  • Weiss RH. 2008. CFT 20-R Grundintelligenztest Skala 2 - revision. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
  • Zajenkowski M, Czarna A. 2015. What makes narcissists unhappy? Subjectively assessed intelligence moderates the relationship between narcissism and psychological well-being. Pers Individ Dif. 77:50–54.
  • Zajenkowski M, Gignac G. 2018. Why do angry people overestimate their intelligence? Neuroticism as a suppressor of the association between trait-anger and subjectively assessed intelligence. Intelligence. 70:12–21.
  • Zajenkowski M, Matthews G. 2019. Intellect and openness differentially predict affect: perceived and objective cognitive ability contexts. Pers Individ Dif. 137:1–8.
  • Zajenkowski M, Stolarski M, Maciantowicz O, Malesza M, Witowska J. 2016. Time to be smart: uncovering a complex interplay between intelligence and time perspectives. Intelligence. 58:1–9.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.