Publication Cover
Chronobiology International
The Journal of Biological and Medical Rhythm Research
Volume 41, 2024 - Issue 3
321
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Article

Circadian modulation of the time course of automatic and controlled semantic processing

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 378-392 | Received 09 May 2023, Accepted 26 Jan 2024, Published online: 05 Feb 2024

References

  • Adan A, Almirall H. 1991. Horne & Östberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire: a reduced scale. Pers Indiv Differ. 12:241–253. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90110-W.
  • Adan A, Archer SN, Hidalgo MP, Di Milia L, Natale V, Randler C. 2012. Circadian typology: a comprehensive review. Chronobiol Int. 29:1153–1175. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.719971.
  • Antypa N, Vogelzangs N, Meesters Y, Schoevers R, Penninx BWJH. 2016. Chronotype associations with depression and anxiety disorders in a large cohort study: research article: chronotypes in depression and anxiety. Depress Anxiety. 33:75–83. doi: 10.1002/da.22422.
  • Balota DA, Black SR, Cheney M. 1992. Automatic and attentional priming in young and older adults: reevaluation of the two-process model. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 18:485–502. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.18.2.485.
  • Becker CA. 1980. Semantic context effects in visual word recognition: an analysis of semantic strategies. Mem Cognit. 8:493–512. doi: 10.3758/BF03213769.
  • Besner D, Humphreys GW. 1991. Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition. L. Erlbaum Associates. http://site.ebrary.com/id/10630126.
  • Blatter K, Cajochen C. 2007. Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: methodological constraints, protocols, theoretical underpinnings. Physiol Behav. 90:196–208. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.009.
  • Bocanegra BR, Hommel B. 2014. When cognitive control is not adaptive. Psychol Sci. 25:1249–1255. doi: 10.1177/0956797614528522.
  • Braver TS. 2012. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends Cogn Sci. 16:106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010.
  • Cheng W-J, Puttonen S, Vanttola P, Koskinen A, Kivimäki M, Härmä M. 2021. Association of shift work with mood disorders and sleep problems according to chronotype: a 17-year cohort study. Chronobiol Int. 38:518–525. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2021.1885431.
  • Collins AM, Loftus EF. 1975. A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychol Rev. 82:407–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407.
  • Correa Á, Molina E, Sanabria D. 2014. Effects of chronotype and time of day on the vigilance decrement during simulated driving. Accident Anal Prev. 67:113–118. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2014.02.020.
  • Daza MT, Ortells JJ, Fox E. 2002. Perception without awareness: further evidence from a Stroop priming task. Percept Psychophys. 64:1316–1324. doi: 10.3758/BF03194774.
  • Di Milia L, Smolensky MH, Costa G, Howarth HD, Ohayon MM, Philip P. 2011. Demographic factors, fatigue, and driving accidents: an examination of the published literature. Accident Anal Prev. 43:516–532. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.12.018.
  • Duffy JF, Rimmer DW, Czeisler CA. 2001. Association of intrinsic circadian period with morningness–eveningness, usual wake time, and circadian phase. Behav Neurosci. 115:895–899. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.4.895.
  • Facer-Childs ER, Campos BM, Middleton B, Skene DJ, Bagshaw AP. 2019. Circadian phenotype impacts the brain’s resting-state functional connectivity, attentional performance, and sleepiness. Sleep. 42:zsz033. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz033.
  • Facer-Childs ER, Middleton B, Skene DJ, Bagshaw AP. 2019. Resetting the late timing of ‘night owls’ has a positive impact on mental health and performance. Sleep Med. 60:236–247. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.05.001.
  • Fales CL, Barch DM, Burgess GC, Schaefer A, Mennin DS, Gray JR, Braver TS. 2008. Anxiety and cognitive efficiency: differential modulation of transient and sustained neural activity during a working memory task. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 8:239–253. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.3.239.
  • Fisk AD, Schneider W. 1981. Control and automatic processing during tasks requiring sustained attention: a new approach to vigilance. Hum Factors. 23:737–750. doi: 10.1177/001872088102300610.
  • Ghin F, Stock A-K, Beste C. 2022. The importance of resource allocation for the interplay between automatic and cognitive control in response inhibition – an EEG source localization study. Cortex. 155:202–217. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.07.004.
  • Goldin AP, Sigman M, Braier G, Golombek DA, Leone MJ. 2020. Interplay of chronotype and school timing predicts school performance. Nat Hum Behav. 4:387–396. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0820-2.
  • Harrison Y, Horne JA. 1999. One night of sleep loss impairs innovative thinking and flexible decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 78:128–145. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1999.2827.
  • Horne JA. 1993. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour: Implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Brit J Psychiat. 162:413–419. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.3.413.
  • JASP Team. 2022. JASP (Version 0.16.3)[Computer software]. [Software]. https://jasp-stats.org/.
  • Kerkhof GA, Van Dongen HPA. 1996. Morning-type and evening-type individuals differ in the phase position of their endogenous circadian oscillator. Neurosci Lett. 218:153–156. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(96)13140-2.
  • Langley LK, Saville AL, Gayzur ND, Fuentes LJ. 2008. Adult age differences in attention to semantic context. Aging Neuropsychol Cognit. 15:657–686. doi: 10.1080/13825580802036928.
  • Lara T, Madrid JA, Correa Á, van Wassenhove V. 2014. The vigilance decrement in executive function is attenuated when individual chronotypes perform at their optimal time of day. PLoS One. 9:e88820. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088820.
  • Lerner I, Bentin S, Shriki O. 2014. Integrating the automatic and the controlled: strategies in semantic priming in an attractor network with latching dynamics. Cogn Sci. 38:1562–1603. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12133.
  • Levandovski R, Sasso E, Hidalgo MP. 2013. Chronotype: a review of the advances, limits and applicability of the main instruments used in the literature to assess human phenotype. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 35:3–11. doi: 10.1590/S2237-60892013000100002.
  • Manly T. 2002. Coffee in the cornflakes: time-of-day as a modulator of executive response control. Neuropsychologia. 40:1–6. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00086-0.
  • Martínez-Pérez V, Palmero LB, Campoy G, Fuentes LJ. 2020. The role of chronotype in the interaction between the alerting and the executive control networks. Sci Rep. 10:11901. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68755-z.
  • Martínez-Pérez V, Tortajada M, Palmero LB, Campoy G, Fuentes LJ. 2022. Effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation over right-DLPFC on vigilance tasks depend on the arousal level. Sci Rep. 12:547. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-04607-8.
  • May CP, Hasher L. 1998. Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 24:363–379. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.2.363.
  • May CP, Hasher L. 2017. Synchrony affects performance for older but not younger neutral-type adults. Timing Time Percept. 5:129–148. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002087.
  • May CP, Hasher L, Foong N. 2005. Implicit memory, age, and time of day: paradoxical priming effects. Psychol Sci. 16:96–100. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00788.x.
  • McNamara TP. 2005. Semantic priming: Perspectives from memory and word recognition. Hove, England: Psychology Press. doi: 10.4324/9780203338001.
  • Merikle PM, Joordens S. 1997. Parallels between perception without attention and perception without awareness. Conscious Cogn. 6:219–236. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1997.0310.
  • Meyer DE, Schvaneveldt RW. 1971. Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. J Exp Psychol. 90:227–234. doi: 10.1037/h0031564.
  • Miller EK, Cohen JD. 2001. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167.
  • Miller JD, Morin LP, Schwartz WJ, Moore RY. 1996. New insights into the mammalian circadian clock. Sleep. 19:641–667. doi: 10.1093/sleep/19.8.641.
  • Molina E, Sanabria D, Jung T-P, Correa Á. 2019. Electroencephalographic and peripheral temperature dynamics during a prolonged psychomotor vigilance task. Accident Anal Prev. 126:198–208. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.10.014.
  • Neely JH. 1977. Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. J Exp Psychol. 106:226–254. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.106.3.226.
  • Neely JH, Keefe DE, Ross KL. 1989. Semantic priming in the lexical decision task: roles of prospective prime-generated expectancies and retrospective semantic matching. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 15:1003–1019. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.15.6.1003.
  • Neely JH, O’Connor PA, Calabrese G. 2010. Fast trial pacing in a lexical decision task reveals a decay of automatic semantic activation. Acta Psychol. 133:127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.11.001.
  • Oginska H, Fafrowicz M, Golonka K, Marek T, Mojsa-Kaja J, Tucholska K. 2010. Chronotype, sleep loss, and diurnal pattern of salivary cortisol in a simulated daylong driving. Chronobiol Int. 27:959–974. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2010.489412.
  • Orban C, Kong R, Li J, Chee MWL, Yeo BTT. 2020. Time of day is associated with paradoxical reductions in global signal fluctuation and functional connectivity. ne Biol. 18:e3000602. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000602.
  • Ortells JJ, Daza MT, Fox E. 2003. Semantic activation in the absence of perceptual awareness. Percept Psychophys. 65:1307–1317. doi: 10.3758/BF03194854.
  • Palmero LB, Martínez-Pérez V, Tortajada M, Campoy G, Fuentes LJ. 2022. Mid-luteal phase progesterone effects on vigilance tasks are modulated by women’s chronotype. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 140:105722. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105722.
  • Palmero LB, Martínez-Pérez V, Tortajada M, Campoy G, Fuentes LJ. 2024. Testing the modulation of self-related automatic and others-related controlled processing by chronotype and time-of-day. Conscious Cogn. 118:103633. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2023.103633.
  • Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA. 2016. Psychology software tools, inc. [E-Prime 3.0]. [ Software]. https://support.pstnet.com/.
  • R Core Team. 2022. A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. [ Software]. https://www.R-project.org/.
  • Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, Kantermann T, Allebrandt K, Gordijn M, Merrow M. 2007. Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev. 11:429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.005.
  • Salehinejad MA, Wischnewski M, Ghanavati E, Mosayebi-Samani M, Kuo M-F, Nitsche MA. 2021. Cognitive functions and underlying parameters of human brain physiology are associated with chronotype. Nat Commun. 12:4672. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-24885-0.
  • Sarabia JA, Rol MA, Mendiola P, Madrid JA. 2008. Circadian rhythm of wrist temperature in normal-living subjects. Physiol Behav. 95:570–580. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.08.005.
  • Schmidt C, Collette F, Cajochen C, Peigneux P. 2007. A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cogn Neuropsychol. 24:755–789. doi: 10.1080/02643290701754158.
  • Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Leclercq Y, Sterpenich V, Vandewalle G, Phillips C, Berthomier P, Berthomier C, Tinguely G, Gais S, et al. 2012. Circadian preference modulates the neural substrate of conflict processing across the day. PLoS One. 7:e29658. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029658.
  • Schneider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A. 2012. E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [ Software].
  • Schneider W, Shiffrin RM. 1977. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol Rev. 84:1–66. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1.
  • Venkat N, Sinha M, Sinha R, Ghate J, Pande B. 2020. Neuro-cognitive profile of morning and evening chronotypes at different times of day. Ann Neurosci. 27:257–265. doi: 10.1177/0972753121990280.
  • Walsh NA, Repa LM, Garland SN. 2022. Mindful larks and lonely owls: the relationship between chronotype, mental health, sleep quality, and social support in young adults. J Sleep Res. 31. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13442.
  • Wolff N, Chmielewski W, Buse J, Roessner V, Beste C. 2019. Paradoxical response inhibition advantages in adolescent obsessive compulsive disorder result from the interplay of automatic and controlled processes. NeuroImage Clin. 23:101893. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101893.
  • Yang L, Hasher L, Wilson DE. 2007. Synchrony effects in automatic and controlled retrieval. Psychon Bull Rev. 14:51–56. doi: 10.3758/BF03194027.