1,100
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Normal or tingly? A story about hands and feet

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 46-54 | Received 29 Sep 2021, Accepted 11 Oct 2021, Published online: 27 Oct 2021

References

  • Ackerley R, Carlsson I, Wester H, Olausson H, Backlund Wasling H. 2014. Touch perceptions across skin sites: differences between sensitivity, direction discrimination and pleasantness. Front Behav Neurosci. 8:54.
  • Association WM. 2013. [email protected]. 2013–2016.
  • Bauer CCC, Barrios FA, Díaz JL. 2014. Subjective somatosensory experiences disclosed by focused attention: cortical-hippocampal-insular and amygdala contributions. PLOS One. 9(8):e104721.
  • Beaudoin R, Michael GA. 2014. Gating of spontaneous somatic sensations by movement. Somatosens Mot Res. 31(3):111–121.
  • Borg C, Emond FC, Colson D, Laurent B, Michael GA. 2015. Attentional focus on subjective interoceptive experience in patients with fibromyalgia. Brain Cogn. 101:35–43.
  • Brusa F, Kretzschmar L, Magnani FG, Turner G, Garraffa M, Sedda A. 2021. Talking with hands: body representation in British sign language users. Exp Brain Res. 239(3):731–744.
  • Cameron JJ, Stinson DA. 2019. Gender (mis)measurement: guidelines for respecting gender diversity in psychological research. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 13(11):1–14.
  • Carey M, Preston C. 2019. Investigating the components of body image disturbance within eating disorders. Front Psychiatry. 10:1–15.
  • Coren S. 1993. The lateral preference inventory for measurement of handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness: norms for young adults. Bull Psychon Soc. 31(1):1–3.
  • Gallagher M, Colzi C, Sedda A. 2021. Dissociation of proprioceptive drift and feelings of ownership in the somatic rubber hand illusion. Acta Psychol. 212:103192.
  • Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JLK. 1998. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 74(6):1464–1480.
  • Gumble A, Carels R. 2012. The harmful and beneficial impacts of weight bias on well-being: the moderating influence of weight status. Body Image. 9(1):101–107.
  • Harris LR, Carnevale MJ, D'Amour S, Fraser LE, Harrar V, Hoover AE, Mander C, Pritchett LM. 2015. How our body influences our perception of the world. Front Psychol. 6:819.
  • Hertzog MA. 2008. Considerations in determining sample size for pilot studies. Res Nurs Health. 31(2):180–191.
  • Karroum EG, Golmard JL, Leu-Semenescu S, Arnulf I. 2012. Sensations in restless legs syndrome. Sleep Med. 13(4):402–408.
  • Michael GA, Tapiero I, Gálvez-García G, Jacquot L. 2017. Thoughts and sensations, twin galaxies of the inner space: the propensity to mind-wander relates to spontaneous sensations arising on the hands. Conscious Cogn. 55:223–231.
  • Michael GA, Dupuy MA, Deleuze A, Humblot M, Simon B, Naveteur J. 2012. Interacting effects of vision and attention in perceiving spontaneous sensations arising on the hands. Exp Brain Res. 216(1):21–34.
  • Michael GA, Guyot D, Tarroux E, Comte M, Salgues S. 2020. Feeling oneself requires embodiment: insights from the relationship between own-body transformations, schizotypal personality traits, and spontaneous bodily sensations. Front Psychol. 11:578237.
  • Michael GA, Naveteur J. 2011. The tickly homunculus and the origins of spontaneous sensations arising on the hands. Conscious Cogn. 20(3):603–617.
  • Michael GA, Naveteur J, Dupuy MA, Jacquot L. 2015. My heart is in my hands: the interoceptive nature of the spontaneous sensations felt on the hands. Physiol Behav. 143:113–120.
  • Naveteur J, Dupuy MA, Gabrielli F, Michael GA. 2015. How we perceive our own hands: effects of attention, aging, and sex. Somatosens Mot Res. 32(4):227–235.
  • Naveteur J, Honoré J, Michael GA. 2005. How to detect an electrocutaneous shock which is not delivered? Overt spatial attention influences decision. Behav Brain Res. 165(2):254–261.
  • Nosek BA, Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. 2005. Understanding and using the implicit association test: II. Method variables and construct validity. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 31(2):166–180.
  • Penfield W, Rasmussen T. 1950. The cerebral cortex of man; a clinical study of localization of function. New York (NY): Macmillan.
  • Preston C, Ehrsson H. 2018. Implicit and explicit changes in body satisfaction evoked by body size illusions: implications for eating disorder vulnerability in women. PLOS One. 13(6):e0199426.
  • Salgues S, Plancher G, Michael GA. 2021a. Visuospatial working memory abilities and spontaneous sensations perception. Somatosens Mot Res. 38(3):164–177.
  • Salgues S, Plancher G, Jacquot L, Naveteur J, Fanuel L, Gálvez-García, Michael GA. 2021b. To the self and beyond: arousal and functional connectivity of the temporo-parietal junction contributes to spontaneous sensations perception. Behav Brain Res. 396:112880.
  • Scarpina F, Magnani FG, Tagini S, Priano L, Mauro A, Sedda A. 2019. Mental representation of the body in action in Parkinson's disease. Exp Brain Res. 237(10):2505–2521.
  • Velkoff EA, Smith AR. 2020. Preliminary development of an implicit association test to measure body dissatisfaction and predict disordered eating behaviors. Body Image. 34:51–58.