Publication Cover
Child Neuropsychology
A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence
Volume 26, 2020 - Issue 6
2,779
Views
24
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

A hierarchical model of cognitive flexibility in children: Extending the relationship between flexibility, creativity and academic achievement

&
Pages 770-800 | Received 25 Jul 2019, Accepted 27 Dec 2019, Published online: 08 Jan 2020

References

  • Agostino, A., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2010). Executive functions underlying multiplicative reasoning: Problem type matters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 286–305.
  • Aguilar-Alonso, Á., & Moreno-González, V. (2012). Neuropsychological differences between samples of dyslexic and reader children by means of NEPSY. The UB Journal of Psychology, 42, 35–50.
  • Aguilar-Alonso, Á., Torres-Viñals, M., & Aguilar-Mediavilla, E. M. (2014). The first Spanish version of the NEPSY for the assessment of the neuropsychological development in a sample of Spanish children. The UB Journal of Psychology, 44, 185–198.
  • Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 85–106.
  • Altemeier, L., Jones, J., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2006). Executive functions in becoming writing readers and reading writers: Note taking and report writing in third and fifth graders. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 161–173.
  • Anderson, V. (2001). Assessing executive functions in children: Biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 4(3), 119–136.
  • Arán Filippetti, V., & López, M. B. (2014). The role of executive functions in academic competences: An analytical review. In K. P. En Bennett (Ed.), Executive functioning. Role in early learning processes, impairments in neurological disorders and impact of cognitive behavior therapy (pp. 305–322). New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
  • Arán Filippetti, V., & Allegri, R. F. (2011). Verbal fluency in Spanish-speaking children: Analysis model according to task type, clustering, and switching strategies and performance over time. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25, 413–436.
  • Arán Filippetti, V., & López, M. B. (2016). Predictores de la Comprensión Lectora en Niños y Adolescentes: El papel de la Edad, el Sexo y las Funciones Ejecutivas. Cuadernos De Neuropsicología, 10, 23–44.
  • Arán Filippetti, V., & Richaud, M. C. (2015). Do executive functions predict written composition? Effects beyond age, verbal intelligence and reading comprehension. Acta Neuropsychologica, 13, 331–349.
  • Arán Filippetti, V., & Richaud, M. C. (2017). A structural equation modeling of executive functions, IQ and mathematical skills in primary students: Differential effects on number production, mental calculus and arithmetical problems. Child Neuropsychology, 23, 864–888.
  • Arbuckle, J. L. (2007). Amos 16.0 User’s Guide. Chicago: Amos Development Corporation.
  • Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. (1994). Development of language, memory, and visuospatial abilities in 5- to 12-year-old children using a neuropsychological battery. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 97–120.
  • Atkinson, T. M., & Ryan, J. P. (2008). The use of variants of the trail making test in serial assessment: A construct validity study. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 26(1), 42–53.
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 47–90). New York: Academic Press.
  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65.
  • Beaty, R. E., Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Jauk, E., & Benedek, M. (2014). The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory & Cognition, 42, 1186–1197.
  • Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(4), 480–485.
  • Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46, 73–83.
  • Benedek, M., Könen, T., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Associative abilities underlying creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(3), 273.
  • Benton, A. L., & Hamsher, K. (1989). Multilingual aphasia examination manual. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.
  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29(3), 180–200.
  • Blackwell, K. A., Cepeda, N. J., & Munakata, Y. (2009). When simple things are meaningful: Working memory strength predicts children’s cognitive flexibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 241–249.
  • Blakey, E., Visser, I., & Carroll, D. J. (2016). Different executive functions support different kinds of cognitive flexibility: Evidence from 2‐, 3‐, and 4‐year‐olds. Child Development, 87, 513–526.
  • Bock, A. M., Gallaway, K. C., & Hund, A. M. (2015). Specifying links between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle childhood: Cognitive flexibility predicts social understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 509–521.
  • Bodimeade, H. L., Whittingham, K., Lloyd, O., & Boyd, R. N. (2013). Executive function in children and adolescents with unilateral cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 55(10), 926–933.
  • Bonino, S., & Cattelino. (1999). The relationship between cognitive abilities and social abilities in childhood: A Research on flexibility in thinking and co-operation with peers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 19–36.
  • Boone, K. B., Pontón, M. O., Gorsuch, R. L., González, J. J., & Miller, B. L. (1998). Factor analysis of four measures of prefrontal lobe functioning. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, 585–595.
  • Brickenkamp, R. (1962). Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-test (Test d2) [The d2 test of attention] (1st ed.). [Spanish adaptation by Seisdedos Cubero, N. (2004); 2nd ed., revised and expanded. Madrid: TEA Ediciones]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
  • Brydges, C. R., Clunies-Ross, K., Clohessy, M., Lo, Z. L., Nguyen, A., Rousset, C., … Fox, A. M. (2012). Dissociable components of cognitive control: An event-related potential (ERP) study of response inhibition and interference suppression. PloS One, 7, e34482.
  • Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 273–293.
  • Carroll, D. J., Blakey, E., & FitzGibbon, L. (2016). Cognitive flexibility in young children: Beyond perseveration. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 211–215.
  • Cepeda, N. J., & Munakata, Y. (2007). Why do children perseverate when they seem to know better: Graded working memory or directed inhibition? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 1058–1065.
  • Chevalier, N., Sheffield, T. D., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2012). Underpinnings of the costs of flexibility in preschool children: The roles of working memory and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37, 99–118.
  • Chi, M. T. H. (1997). Creativity: Shifting across ontological categories flexibly. In T. B. Ward, S. M. Smith, & J. Vaid (Eds.), Creative thought. An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (pp. 209–234). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10227-009
  • Clapham, M. M., & King, W. R. (2010). Psychometric characteristics of the CREA in an English speaking population. Anales De Psicología/Annals of Psychology, 26, 206–211.
  • Cools, R., Brouwer, W. H., de Jong, R., & Slooff, C. (2000). Flexibility, inhibition, and planning: Frontal dysfunctioning in schizophrenia. Brain and Cognition, 43(1–3), 108–112.
  • Corbalán Berná, J., Martínez Zaragoza, F., Donolo, D., Alonso Monreal, C., Tejerina Arreal, M., & Limiñana Gras, R. M. (2003). CREA. Inteligencia creativa. Una medida cognitiva de la creatividad. España: TEA ediciones.
  • Costantini, A. F., & Hoving, K. L. (1973). The relationship of cognitive and motor response inhibition to age and IQ. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 123, 309–319.
  • Cragg, L., & Chevalier, N. (2012). The processes underlying flexibility in childhood. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(2), 209–232.
  • Cuetos Vega, F., Ramos Sánchez, J. L., & Ruano Hernández, E. (2004). PROESC. Evaluación de los procesos de escritura. Madrid: TEA ediciones.
  • Dajani, D. R., & Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends in Neurosciences, 38, 571–578.
  • De Dreu, C. K. D., Nijstad, B. A., & Baas, M. (2011). Behavioral activation links to creativity because of increased cognitive flexibility. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(1), 72–80.
  • De Dreu, C. K. W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation in the mood–Creativity link: Towards a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 739–756.
  • Deák, G. O. (2003). The development of cognitive flexibility and language abilities. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31, pp. 273–328). San Diego: Academic Press.
  • Deák, G. O., & Wiseheart, M. (2015). Cognitive flexibility in young children: General or task-specific capacity? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138, 31–53.
  • Demagistri, M. S., Richards, M. M., & Canet-Juric, L. (2014). Incidencia del Funcionamiento Ejecutivo en el Rendimiento en Comprensión Lectora en Adolescentes. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 12, 343–370.
  • Diamond, A. (2006). The early development of executive functions. In E. Bialystok & F. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 70–95). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195169539.003.0006
  • Diamond, A. (2016). Why improving and assessing executive functions early in life is critical. In J. A. Griffin, P. McCardle, & L. S. Freund (Eds.), Executive function in pre-school age children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research. Washington, DC: APA. doi:10.1037/14797-002
  • Duan, X., Wei, S., Wang, G., & Shi, J. (2010). The relationship between executive functions and intelligence on 11-to 12-year-old children. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 52(4), 419.
  • Ebersbach, M., & Hagedorn, H. (2011). The role of cognitive flexibility in the spatial representation of children’s drawings. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 32–55.
  • Engel de Abreu, P. M., Abreu, N., Nikaedo, C. C., Puglisi, M. L., Tourinho, C. J., Miranda, M. C., … Martin, R. (2014). Executive functioning and reading achievement in school: A study of Brazilian children assessed by their teachers as “poor readers”. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 550.
  • Eslinger, P. J., Biddle, K., Pennington, B., & Page, R. B. (1999). Cognitive and behavioral development up to 4 years after early right frontal lobe lesion. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15, 157–191.
  • Eslinger, P. J., & Grattan, L. M. (1993). Frontal lobe and frontal-striatal substrates for different forms of human cognitive flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 31, 17–28.
  • Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M. M., Hamby, A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 465–486.
  • Fenesy, M. C., & Lee, S. S. (2018). Executive functioning mediates predictions of youth academic and social development from parenting behavior. Developmental Neuropsychology, 43(8), 729–750.
  • Fisk, J. E., & Sharp, C. A. (2004). Age-related impairment in executive functioning: Updating, inhibition, shifting, and access. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 874–890.
  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190.
  • Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 611–625.
  • Golden, C. J. (1975). The measurement of creativity by the Stroop color and word test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 39, 502–506.
  • Golden, C. J. (1999). Stroop, Test de Colores y Palabras. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Greve, K. W., Stickle, T. R., Love, J. M., Bianchini, K. J., & Stanford, M. S. (2005). Latent structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A confirmatory factor analytic study. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 355–364.
  • Gyurak, A., Goodkind, M. S., Madan, A., Kramer, J. H., Miller, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (2009). Do tests of executive functioning predict ability to downregulate emotions spontaneously and when instructed to suppress? Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(2), 144–152.
  • Haut, M. W., Kuwabara, H., Leach, S., & Arias, R. G. (2000). Neural activation during performance of number–Letter sequencing. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 7, 237–242.
  • Hayes, J. R. (1989). Cognitive Processes in Creativity. In J. A. Glove, R. R. Ronnin, & C. R. Reynold (Eds.), Handbook of Creativity (pp. 135–145). Boston, MA: Plenum Press, Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4757-5356-
  • Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST). [Spanish adaptation by de la Cruz López, M. V. (1997). Madrid: TEA Ediciones]. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance following focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychology, 18, 284–295.
  • Holt, A. E., & Deák, G. O. (2015). Children’s task-switching efficiency: Missing our cue? Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 261–285.
  • Hooper, S. R., Swartz, C. W., Wakely, M. B., de Kruif, R. E., & Montgomery, J. W. (2002). Executive functions in elementary school children with and without problems in written expression. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 57–68.
  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 76–99). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55.
  • Hughes, C. (2011). Changes and challenges in 20 years of research into the development of executive functions. Infant and Child Development, 20(3), 251–271.
  • Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2017–2036.
  • Hurks, P. P., Schrans, D., Meijs, C., Wassenberg, R., Feron, F. J. M., & Jolles, J. (2010). Developmental changes in semantic verbal fluency: Analyses of word productivity as a function of time, clustering, and switching. Child Neuropsychology, 16(4), 366–387.
  • Im‐Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual‐Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77(6), 1822–1841.
  • Ionescu, T. (2012). Exploring the nature of cognitive flexibility. New Ideas in Psychology, 30, 190–200.
  • Ionescu, T. (2017). When is a cognitive system flexible? The variability–stability–flexibility pattern on the way to novel solutions. AVANT. Pismo Awangardy Filozoficzno-Naukowej, S, 255–264.
  • Jacobson, L. A., Williford, A. P., & Pianta, R. C. (2011). The role of executive function in children’s competent adjustment to middle school. Child Neuropsychology, 17, 255–280.
  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2000). K-BIT. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. [Spanish adaptation by Cordero A. & Calonge, I. Madrid: TEA Ediciones]. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
  • Kim, Y. J., & Zhong, C. B. (2017). Ideas rise from chaos: Information structure and creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 138, 15–27.
  • Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3-to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 407–428.
  • Korkman, M., Kemp, S. L., & Kirk, U. (2001). Effects of age on neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: A cross-sectional study on 800 children from the United States. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 331–354.
  • Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. L. (1998). NEPSY. A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
  • Kortte, K. B., Horner, M. D., & Windham, W. K. (2002). The trail making test, part B: Cognitive flexibility or ability to maintain set? Applied Neuropsychology, 9, 106–109.
  • Kozulin, A. (1993). Literature as a psychological tool. Educational Psychologist, 28(3), 253–264.
  • Krumm, G., Arán Filippetti, V., & Lemos, V. (2018). Correlates between two measures to assess creativity in Argentine children: CREA and TTCT. In J. C. Penagos Corzo & M. A. Padilla Vargas (Eds.), Challenges in creativity & psychology for the XXI century (pp. 46–60). México: Fundación de las Américas, Puebla.
  • Krumm, G., Arán Filippetti, V., & Gutierrez, M. (2018). The contribution of Executive Functions to Creativity in Children: What is the role of Crystallized and Fluid Intelligence? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 29, 185–195.
  • Krumm, G., Arán Filippetti, V., Lemos, V., Koval, J., & Balabanian, C. (2016). Construct validity and factorial invariance across sex of the torrance test of creative thinking–figural form A in Spanish-speaking children. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 180–189.
  • Lee, C. S., & Therriault, D. J. (2013). The cognitive underpinnings of creative thought: A latent variable analysis exploring the roles of intelligence and working memory in three creative thinking processes. Intelligence, 41(5), 306–320.
  • Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59–80.
  • Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., & Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. USA: Oxford University Press.
  • Lindqvist, G. (2003). Vygotsky’s theory of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 15(2–3), 245–251.
  • Locascio, G., Mahone, E. M., Eason, S., & Cutting, L. (2010). Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension deficits. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 441–454.
  • López Martínez, O., & Navarro Lozano, J. (2008). Estudio comparativo entre medidas de creatividad: TTCT vs. CREA. Anales De Psicología, 24, 138–142.
  • Mainville, M., Brisson, J., Nougarou, F., Stipanicic, A., & Sirois, S. (2015). Inhibition development: Comparison of neuropsychological and eye tracking measures. Revista Argentina De Ciencias Del Comportamiento, 7, 17–25.
  • Marcovitch, S., Boseovski, J. J., Knapp, R. J., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Goal neglect and working memory capacity in 4‐to 6‐year‐old children. Child Development, 81, 1687–1695.
  • Mardia, K. V. (1970). Measures of multivariate skewness and kurtosis with applications. Biometrika, 57, 519–530.
  • Matute, E., Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., & Morales, G. (2004). Verbal and nonverbal fluency in Spanish-speaking children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(2), 647–660.
  • Matute, E., Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., & Ostrosky-Solís, F. (2007). Evaluación neuropsicológica infantil (ENI). México: Manual Moderno.
  • Maylor, E. A., Moulson, J. M., Muncer, A. M., & Taylor, L. A. (2002). Does performance on theory of mind tasks decline in old age? British Journal of Psychology, 93(4), 465–485.
  • Messer, D., Bernardi, M., Botting, N., Hill, E. L., Nash, G., Leonard, H. C., & Henry, L. A. (2018). An exploration of the factor structure of executive functioning in children. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1179.
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.
  • Monette, S., Bigras, M., & Guay, M. C. (2011). The role of the executive functions in school achievement at the end of Grade 1. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(2), 158–173.
  • Monette, S., Bigras, M., & Lafrenière, M. A. (2015). Structure of executive functions in typically developing kindergarteners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 120–139.
  • Morra, S., Bisagno, E., Caviola, S., Delfante, C., & Mammarella, I. C. (2019). Working memory capacity and the development of quantitative central conceptual structures. Cognition and Instruction, 37(4), 483–511.
  • Morra, S., Panesi, S., Traverso, L., & Usai, M. C. (2018). Which tasks measure what? Reflections on executive function development and a commentary on Podjarny, Kamawar, and Andrews (2017). Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 246–258.
  • Nayfeld, I., Fuccillo, J., & Greenfield, D. B. (2013). Executive functions in early learning: Extending the relationship between executive functions and school readiness to science. Learning and Individual Differences, 26, 81–88.
  • Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence, 39, 36–45.
  • Pan, X., & Yu, H. (2018). Different effects of cognitive shifting and intelligence on creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 52, 212–225.
  • Parkin, A. J., & Lawrence, A. (1994). A dissociation in the relation between memory tasks and frontal lobe tests in the normal elderly. Neuropsychologia, 32(12), 1523–1532.
  • Pozzetti, T., Ometto, A., Gangi, S., Picciolini, O., Presezzi, G., Gardon, L., … Marzocchi, G. M. (2014). Emerging executive skills in very preterm children at 2 years corrected age: A composite assessment. Child Neuropsychology, 20(2), 145–161.
  • Pratt, M. L., Leonard, H. C., Adeyinka, H., & Hill, E. L. (2014). The effect of motor load on planning and inhibition in developmental coordination disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 1579–1587.
  • Regard, M. (1981). Cognitive rigidity and flexibility. A neuropsychological study (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Victoria, British Columbia.
  • Regard, M., Strauss, E., & Knapp, P. (1982). Children’s production on verbal and non-verbal fluency tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 839–844.
  • Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1992). Neuropsychological evaluation of older children. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.
  • Richard’s, M., Canet-Juric, L., Introzzi, I., & Urquijo, S. (2014). Intervención diferencial de las funciones ejecutivas en inferencias elaborativas y puente. Avances En Psicología Latinoamericana, 32, 5–20.
  • Rodríguez-Aranda, C., & Sundet, K. (2006). The frontal hypothesis of cognitive aging: Factor structure and age effects on four frontal tests among healthy individuals. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167, 269–287.
  • Romo, M., Alfonso-Benlliure, V., & Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J. (2016). El test de creatividad infantil (TCI): Evaluando la creatividad mediante una tarea de encontrar problemas. Psicología Educativa, 22, 93–101.
  • Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2012). Implications of infant cognition for executive functions at age 11. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1345–1355.
  • Rosenthal, E. N., Riccio, C. A., Gsanger, K. M., & Pizzitola Jarratt, K. (2006). Digit Span components as predictors of attention problems and executive functioning in children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 131–139.
  • Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Matute, E., & Inozemtseva, O. (2009). Gender differences and cognitive correlates of mathematical skills in school-aged children. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 216–231.
  • Rosselli-Cock, M., Matute-Villaseñor, E., Ardila-Ardila, A., Botero-Gómez, V. E., Tangarife-Salazar, G. A., Echeverría-Pulido, S. E., & Ocampo-Agudelo, P. (2004). Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil (ENI): Una batería para la evaluación de niños entre 5 y 16 años de edad. Estudio normativo colombiano. Revista De Neurología, 38(8), 720–731.
  • Roy, A., Barbarot, S., Roulin, J. L., Charbonnier, V., Fasotti, L., Stalder, J. F., & Le Gall, D. (2014). Is executive function specifically impaired in children with neurofibromatosis type 1? A neuropsychological investigation of cognitive flexibility. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 3(2), 94–102.
  • Runco, M. A. (2008). Commentary: Divergent thinking is not synonymous with creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Art, 2, 93–96.
  • Schiffer, B., Müller, B. W., Scherbaum, N., Forsting, M., Wiltfang, J., Leygraf, N., & Gizewski, E. R. (2010). Impulsivity-related brain volume deficits in schizophrenia-addiction comorbidity. Brain, 133(10), 3093–3103.
  • Sesma, H. W., Mahone, E. M., Levine, T., Eason, S. H., & Cutting, L. E. (2009). The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 232–246.
  • Shaul, S., & Schwartz, M. (2014). The role of the executive functions in school readiness among preschool-age children. Reading and Writing, 27, 749–768.
  • Slachevsky, A., Pérez, C., Silva, J., Orellana, G., Prenafeta, M. L., Alegria, P., & Peña, G. (2005). Córtex prefrontal y trastornos del comportamiento: Modelos explicativos y métodos de evaluación. Revista Chilena De Neuropsiquiatría, 43, 109–121.
  • Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological test: Administration, norms and commentary (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 745–759.
  • Theodoraki, T. E., McGeown, S. P., Rhodes, S. M., & MacPherson, S. E. (2019). Developmental changes in executive functions during adolescence: A study of inhibition, shifting, and working memory. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12307
  • Thorell, L. B., Veleiro, A., Siu, A. F., & Mohammadi, H. (2013). Examining the relation between ratings of executive functioning and academic achievement: Findings from a cross-cultural study. Child Neuropsychology, 19, 630–638.
  • Tiego, J., Testa, R., Bellgrove, M. A., Pantelis, C., & Whittle, S. (2018). A hierarchical model of inhibitory control. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1339.
  • Tirapu Ustárroz, J., Bausela Herreras, E., & Cordero Andrés, P. (2018). Modelo de funciones ejecutivas basado en análisis factoriales en población infantil y escolar: Metaanálisis. Revista De Neurología, 67(6), 215–225.
  • Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Cordero-Andrés, P., & Basuela-Herreras, E. (2018). Funciones ejecutivas en población infantil: Propuesta de una clarificación conceptual e integradora basada en resultado de análisis factoriales. Cuadernos De Neuropsicología/Panamerican Journal of Neuropsychology, 12(3). doi:10.7714/CNPS/12.3.203
  • Tomer, R., Fisher, T., Giladi, N., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2002). Dissociation between spontaneous and reactive flexibility in early Parkinson’s disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 15, 106–112.
  • Torrance, P., Ball, O., & Safter, H. T. (1992). Torrance test of creative thinking. Streamlined scoring guide figural A and B. Bensenville, Illinois: Scholastic Testing Service.
  • Tucha, L., Aschenbrenner, S., Koerts, J., & Lange, K. W. (2012). The five-point test: Reliability, validity and normative data for children and adults. PloS One, 7, e46080.
  • van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2004). Inhibition and shifting in children with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(3), 239–266.
  • van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35, 427–449.
  • van der Ven, S. H., Kroesbergen, E. H., Boom, J., & Leseman, P. P. (2013). The structure of executive functions in children: A closer examination of inhibition, shifting, and updating. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(1), 70–87.
  • Viterbori, P., Usai, M. C., Traverso, L., & De Franchis, V. (2015). How preschool executive functioning predicts several aspects of math achievement in grades 1 and 3: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 38–55.
  • Vuontela, V., Steenari, M. R., Carlson, S., Koivisto, J., Fjällberg, M., & Aronen, E. T. (2003). Audiospatial and visuospatial working memory in 6–13 year old school children. Learning & Memory, 10(1), 74–81.
  • Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth edition (WISC-IV). [Spanish adaptation (2005), Madrid: TEA Ediciones]. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Wechsler, D. (2010). WISC IV, Escala de Inteligencia para niños de Wechsler – IV. Adaptación Argentina. Buenos Aires: Paidos.
  • Willoughby, M., & Blair, C. (2011). Test-retest reliability of a new executive function battery for use in early childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 17(6), 564–579.
  • Wu, K. K., Chan, S. K., Leung, P. W., Liu, W. S., Leung, F. L., & Ng, R. (2011). Components and developmental differences of executive functioning for school-aged children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(3), 319–337.
  • Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Pieper, S. (2013). Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in children: A meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 1–9.
  • Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 136–143.
  • Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W., & Salvendy, G. (2011). Can traditional divergent thinking tests be trusted in measuring and predicting real-world creativity? Creativity Research Journal, 23, 24–37.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.