567
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Pluralism, social cognition, and interaction in autism

Pages 161-184 | Received 16 Feb 2016, Accepted 24 Oct 2016, Published online: 28 Nov 2016

References

  • Andrews, K. (2012). Do apes read minds?. London: MIT Press.
  • Apperly, I. (2008). Beyond simulation-theory and theory-theory: Why social cognitive neuroscience should use its own concepts to study “Theory of Mind”. Cognition, 107, 2662–2683.
  • Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320.10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311
  • Bahrick, L. R., & Watson, J. S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963–973.10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.963
  • Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110–118.10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Do people with autism understand what causes emotion? Child Development, 62, 385–395.10.2307/1131011
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Theory of mind and autism: A fifteen year review. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 3–20). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., & Howlin, P. (1993). The theory of mind deficit in autism: Some questions for teaching and diagnosis. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 4664–4680). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8
  • Baron-cohen, S., Spitz, A., & Cross, P. (1993). Do children with autism recognize surprise? Cognition and Emotion, 7, 507–516.10.1080/02699939308409202
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Walker, J. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 379–398.10.1111/bjdp.1995.13.issue-4
  • Betz, A., Higbee, T. S., & Reagon, K. A. (2008). Using joint activity schedules to promote peer engagement in pre-schoolers with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 237–241.10.1901/jaba.2008.41-237
  • Bowler, D. M. (1992). Theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 877–893.10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb01962.x
  • Bryson, S. E., Landry, R., Czapinski, P., McConnell, B., Rombough, V., & Wainwright, A. (2004). Autistic spectrum disorders: Causal mechanisms and recent findings on attention and emotion. International Journal of Special Education, 19, 14–22.
  • Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 589–599.10.1023/A:1013251112392
  • Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Austism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.10.1093/brain/awf189
  • Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377–395.10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4
  • Csibra, G. (2007). Action mirroring and action understanding: An alternative account. In: P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition. Attention and performance XII (pp. 453–459). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2007). ‘Obsessed with goals’: Functions and mechanisms of teleological interpretation of actions in humans. Acta Psychologica, 124, 607–608.
  • Cusack, J. P., Williams, J. H. G., & Neri, P. (2015). Action perception is intact in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 1849–1857.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4133-13.2015
  • Da Fonseca, D., Santos, A., Rosset, D., & Deruelle, C. (2011). The “beauty is good” for children with autism spectrum disorders too. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 299–304.10.1016/j.rasd.2010.04.012
  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.10.1038/nn1611
  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283.10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271
  • Diaconescu, A. O., Mathys, C., Weber, L. A. E., Daunizeau, J., Kasper, L., Lomakina, E. I., … Stephan, K. E. (2014). Inferring on the intentions of others by hierachical Bayesian learning. PLOS Computational Biology, 10(9), 1–19.
  • Dumas, G., Soussignan, R., Hugueville, L., Martinerie, J., & Nadel, J. (2014). Revisiting mu suppression in autism spectrum disorder. Brain Research, 1585, 108–119.10.1016/j.brainres.2014.08.035
  • Egyed, K., Kiraly, I., & Gergely, G. (2013). Communicating shared knowledge in infancy. Psychological Science, 24, 1348–1353.10.1177/0956797612471952
  • Fiebich, A. (2015). Varieties of social understanding. Paderborn: Mentis.
  • Fiebich, A. (2016). Narratives, culture, and folk psychology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 15, 135–149.10.1007/s11097-014-9378-7
  • Fiebich, A., & Coltheart, M. (2015). Various ways to understand other minds: Towards a pluralistic approach to the explanation of social understanding. Mind and Language, 30, 235–258.10.1111/mila.2015.30.issue-3
  • Fiebich, A., Gallagher, S., & Hutto, D. (2017). Pluralism, interaction and the ontogeny of social cognition. In J. Kiverstein (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of the philosophy of the social mind (pp. 208–221). New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Fisher, N., & Happé, F. (2005). A training study of theory of mind and executive function in children with autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 757–771.10.1007/s10803-005-0022-9
  • Fivush, R., & Hamond, N. (1990). Autobiographical memory across the preschool years: Towards reconceptualizing childhood amnesia. In R. Fivush & J. A. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 223–248). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Frith, U. (2003). Autism—explaining the enigma (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Frith, U. (2004). Emanuel Miller lecture: Confusions and controversies about Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 672–686.10.1111/jcpp.2004.45.issue-4
  • Gallagher, S. (2004). Understanding interpersonal problems in autism: Interaction theory as an alternative to theory of mind. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology, 11, ;–217.10.1353/ppp.2004.0063
  • Gallagher, S. (2007). Simulation trouble. Social Neuroscience, 2, 353–365.
  • Gallagher, S. (2015). The new hybrids: Continuing debates on social perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 452–465.10.1016/j.concog.2015.04.002
  • Gallagher, S., & Varga, S. (2015). Social cognition and psychopathology: A critical overview. World Psychiatry, 14, 5–14.10.1002/wps.20173
  • Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2008). The phenomenological mind: An introduction to philosophy of mind and cognitive science. London: Routledge.
  • Gergely, G. (2001). The obscure object of desire: Nearly, but clearly not, like me’. Contingency preference in normal children versus children with autism. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 65, 411–426.10.1521/bumc.65.3.411.19853
  • Godman, M. (2013). Why we do things together: The social motivation for joint action. Philosophical Psychology, 26, 588–603.10.1080/09515089.2012.670905
  • Goldman, A. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/0195138929.001.0001
  • Gopnik, A. (1998). The scientist as child. In A. Gopnik & A. Meltzoff (Eds.), Words, thoughts, and theories (pp. 13–47). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Grafton, S. T. (2009). Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience, 1156, 97–117.
  • Grossman, J. B., Klin, A., Carter, A. S., & Volkmar, F. R. (2000). Verbal bias in recognition of facial emotions in children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 369–379.10.1111/jcpp.2000.41.issue-3
  • Happé, F. (1994). Autism, an introduction to psychological theory. London: UCL Press.
  • Happé, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M., Gillberg, C., … , & Frith, C. (1996). ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. NeuroReport, 8, 197–201.10.1097/00001756-199612200-00040
  • Hamilton, A. F., Brindley, R., & Frith, U. (2007). Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia, 45, 1859–1868.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.022
  • Hamilton, A. F., Brindley, R., & Frith, U. (2009). Visual perspective taking impairment in children with autistic spectrum disorder. Cognition, 113, 37–44.10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.007
  • Hirschfeld, L., Bartmess, E., White, S., & Frith, U. (2007). Can autistic children predict behavior by social stereotypes? Current Biology, 17, R451–R452.10.1016/j.cub.2007.04.051
  • Hoffman, E. A., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 80–84.10.1038/71152
  • Hudgens-Haney, M. E. (2010). Autism, social comprehension, and cognitive impenetrability. Philosophy Theses. Paper 75. Retrieved from: http://digitalarchive.gsu.edu/philosophy_theses/75
  • Hutto, D. D. (2008). Folk psychological narratives: The sociocultural basis of understanding reasons. Cambridge, MA: Bradford.
  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 358, 345–360.10.1098/rstb.2002.1202
  • Klin, A., David, J. L., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather biological motion. Nature, 459, 257–261.10.1038/nature07868
  • Lee, A., & Hobson, R. P. (1998). On developing self-concepts: A controlled study of children and adolescents with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 1131–1144.10.1111/jcpp.1998.39.issue-8
  • Leekam, S. R., López, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36, 173–261.
  • Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Social cognitive neuroscience. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed.). (pp. 143–193). New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? Cognition, 105, 4895–4912.
  • Luo, Y., & Johnson, S. C. (2009). Recognizing the role of perception in action in at 6 months. Developmental Science, 12, 142–149.10.1111/desc.2009.12.issue-1
  • McGeer, V. (2009). The skill of perceiving persons. The Modern Schoolman, 86, 289–318.10.5840/schoolman2009863/45
  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838
  • Mitchell, R. W., & Hamm, M. (1997). The interpretation of animal psychology: Anthropomorphism or behavior reading? Behaviour, 134, 173–204.10.1163/156853997X00449
  • Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How does it look? Level 2 perspective-taking at 36 months of age. Child Development, 82, 661–673.10.1111/cdev.2011.82.issue-2
  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.10.1007/BF02206861
  • Nadel, J., Croue, S., Mattlinger, M.-J., Canet, P., Hudelot, C., Lecuyer, C., & Martini, M. (2000). Do autistic children have expectancies about the social behaviour of unfamiliar people? A pilot study with the still face paradigm. Autism, 4, 133–145.10.1177/1362361300004002003
  • Nagy, E. (2008). Innate intersubjectivity: Newborns' sensitivity to communication disturbance. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1779–1784.10.1037/a0012665
  • Neufeld, J., Ioannou, C., Korb, S., Schilbach, L., & Chakrabarti, B. (2015). Spontaneous facial mimicry is modulated by joint attention and autistic traits. Autism Research, 9, 781–789.
  • Nyström, P. (2008). The infant mirror neuron system studied with high density EEG. Social Neuroscience, 3, 334–347.10.1080/17470910701563665
  • Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG-evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198.10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014
  • Oppenheim, D. M. (2008). The secret life of fluency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 2372–2441.
  • Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching theory of mind: A new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 415–433.10.1007/BF02179376
  • Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism. Brain, 128, 1038–1048.10.1093/brain/awh404
  • Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2003). Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: The influence of context. Neuropsychologia, 41, 156–170.10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00146-X
  • Perner, J. (1999). Metakognition und introspektion in entwicklungspsychologischer sicht: Studien zur “theory of mind” und “simulation”. In W. Janke & W. Schneider (Eds.), Hundert Jahre Institut für Psychologie und Würzburger Schule der Denkpsychologie (pp. 411–431). Göttingen: Hofgrede.
  • Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (2005). Infants’ insight into the mind: How deep? Science, 308, 214–216.10.1126/science.1111656
  • Pfeiffer, U. J., Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Kuzmanovic, B., Georgescu, A. L., Bente, G., & Vogeley, K. (2014). Why we interact: Activation of the ventral striatum during real-time social interaction. NeuroImage, 101, 124–137.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.061
  • Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The sources of normativity: Young children's awareness of the normative structure of games. Developmental Psychology, 44, 875–881.10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.875
  • Reeder, G. D., & Trafimow, D. (2005). Attributing motives to other people. In B. F. Malle & S. D. Hodges (Eds.), Other minds: How humans bridge the divide between self and others (pp. 106–123). New York: Guilford Press.
  • Reis, H. T., & Wright, S. (1982). Knowledge of sex-role stereotypes in children aged 3 to 5. Sex Roles, 8, 1049–1056.10.1007/BF00290999
  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230
  • Rump, K. M., Joyce, L. G., Minshew, N. J., & Strauss, M. S. (2009). The development of emotion recognition in individuals with autism. Child Development, 80, 1434–1447.10.1111/cdev.2009.80.issue-5
  • Rutherford, M. D., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2006). The perception of animacy in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 983–992.10.1007/s10803-006-0136-8
  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson.
  • Sacks, O. (1995). An Anthropologist on Mars. London: Picador.
  • Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S. B., Cieslik, E. C., Kuzmanovic, B., & Vogeley, K. (2012). Shall we do this together? Social gaze influences action control in a comparison group, but not in individuals with high-functioning autism. Autism, 16, 152–162.
  • Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 393–414.10.1017/S0140525X12000660
  • Schilbach, L., Wohlschlaeger, A. M., Kraemer, N. C., Newen, A., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., & Vogeley, K. (2006). Being with virtual others: Neural correlates of social interaction. Neuropsychologia, 44, 718–730.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.017
  • Schmitz, N., Rubia, K., van Amelsvoort, T., Daly, E., Smith, A., & Murphy, D. G. (2008). Neural correlates of reward in autism. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, 19–24.10.1192/bjp.bp.107.036921
  • Schwarzkopf, S., Schilbach, L., Vogeley, K., & Timmermans, B. (2014). “Making it explicit” makes a difference: Evidence for dissociation of spontaneous and intentional level 1 perspective taking in high-functioning autism. Cognition, 131, 345–354.10.1016/j.cognition.2014.02.003
  • Senju, A. (2012). Spontaneous theory of mind and its absence in autism spectrum disorders. The Neuroscientist, 18, 108–113.10.1177/1073858410397208
  • Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., Kwon, J. H., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807.10.2307/1131234
  • Sinigaglia, C. (2010). Mirroring and understanding action. In M. Suárez, M. Dorato, & M. Rédei (Eds.), EPSA philosophical issues in the sciences (pp. 227–238). Dordrecht: Springer.10.1007/978-90-481-3252-2
  • Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ action. Cognition, 96, B1–B11.10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004
  • Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587–592.10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x
  • Stich, S., & Nichols, S. (1992). Folk psychology: Simulation or tacit theory? Mind and Language, 7, 35–71.10.1111/mila.1992.7.issue-1-2
  • Timmermans, B., & Schilbach, L. (2014). Investigating alterations of social interaction in psychiatric disorders with dual interactive eye tracking and virtual faces. Frontiers in Human Neurosciences, 8, 758.
  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.
  • Vaish, A., & Woodward, A. (2010). Infants use attention but not emotions to predict others’ actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 33, 79–87.10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.11.003
  • Vivanti, G., McCormick, C., Young, G. S., & Rogers, S. J. (2011). Intact and impaired mechanisms of action understanding in autism. Developmental Psychology, 47, 841–856.10.1037/a0023105
  • Watson, J. S. (1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In E. Thoman (Ed.), The origins of social responsiveness (pp. 33–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Wellman, H. M., & Gopnik, A. (1992). Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language, 7, 145–171.
  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.10.1111/cdev.2001.72.issue-3
  • Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
  • Zahavi, D., & Parnas, J. (2003). Conceptual problems in infantile autism research: Why cognitive science needs phenomenology. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 53–71.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.