715
Views
33
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The psychological reality of practical representation

Pages 784-821 | Received 29 Nov 2017, Accepted 02 Apr 2018, Published online: 23 May 2019

References

  • Anderson, J. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89(4), 369.
  • Anderson, J. R. (2005). Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York, NY: Macmillan.
  • Anscombe, G. E. M. (1957). Intentions. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
  • Arbib, M. A. (1981). Perceptual structure and the distributed motor control. In V. B. Brooks (Ed.), Handbook of physiology, section I: The nervous system (Vol. 2, pp. 1449–1480). Motor Control, Baltimore: Williams et Wilkins.
  • Arbib, M. A. (1985). Schemas for the temporal organization of behavior. Human Neurobiology, 4, 63–72.
  • Armstrong, D. (1973). Belief, truth and knowledge. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Barker, C. (2012) Imperatives denote actions. In: Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL, 57–70.
  • Bayley, P., Franscino, J., & Squire, L. (2005). Robust habit learning in the absence of awareness and independent of the medial temporal lobe. Nature, 463(7050), 550–553.
  • Bermúdez, J. L. (1995). Nonconceptual content: From perceptual experience to subpersonal computational states. Mind & Language, 10(4), 333–369.
  • Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
  • Braddon-Mitchell, D., & Jackson, F. (1996). Philosophy of mind and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publish.
  • Brozzo, C. (2017). Motor intentions: How intentions and motor representations come together. Mind and Language, 32(2), 231–256.
  • Burge, T. (2009). "Five theses on de re states and attitudes" in Almog J. and Leonardi P, The Philosophy of David Kaplan. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, pp. 246–324.
  • Burge, T. (2010). Origins of objectivity. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Butterfill, S. A., & Sinigaglia, C. (2014). Intention and motor representation in purposive action. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 88, 119–145.
  • Camp, E. (2009). Putting thoughts to work: Concepts, systematicity, and stimulus-independence. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 78(2), 275–311.
  • Carruthers, P. (2006). The Architecture of Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Cohen, N. J., & Eichenbaum, H. (1993). Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210(4466), 207–210.
  • Coombs, S., Fay, R. R., & Elepfandt, A. (2010). Dipole source encoding and tracking by the goldfish auditory system. Journal of Experimental Biology, 213(20), 3536–3547.
  • Dau, T., Püschel, D., & Kohlrausch, A. (1996). A quantitative model of the “effective”signal processing in the auditory system. I. Model structure. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99(6), 3615–3622.
  • DeBrigard, F. (forthcoming). Know-how, intellectualism, and memory systems. In Philosophical Psychology.
  • Dennett, D. C. (1969). The intentional stance. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
  • Devitt, M. (2011). Methodology and the nature of knowing how. Journal of Philosophy, 108, 205–218.
  • Dew, I. T., & Cabeza, R. (2011). The porous boundaries between explicit and implicit memory: Behavioral and neural evidence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224(1), 174–190.
  • Dretske, F. (1988). Explaining behavior: Reasons in a world of causes. Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press.
  • Epstein, M. L. (1980). The relationship of mental imagery and mental rehearsal to performance of a motor task. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2(3), 211–220.
  • Evans, G. (1982). The variety of reference. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  • Feinberg, I. (1978). Efference copy and corollary discharge: Implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 4(4), 636.
  • Feltz, D. L., & Landers, D. M. (1983). The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport Psychology, 5(1), 25–57.
  • Fodor, J. A. (1968). The appeal to tacit knowledge in psychological explanation. The Journal Of Philosophy, 65(20), 627-640.
  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The Language of Thought. New York, NY: Thomas Y. Crowell.
  • Fodor, J. A. (1994). Concepts: A potboiler. Cognition, 50(1), 95–113.
  • Fodor, J. A. (1998). Concepts: Where cognitive science went wrong. New york, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Foerde, K., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, 11778–11783.
  • Fridland, E. (2017). Skill and motor control: Intelligence all the way down. Synthese, 174(6), 1539–1560.
  • Gallese, V., & Metzinger, T. (2003). Motor ontology: The representational reality of goals, actions and selves. Philosophical Psychology, 16, 365–388.
  • Gallistel. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
  • Gibbons, J. (2001). Knowledge in action. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 62(3), 579–600.
  • Girard, J. Y., Taylor, P., & Lafont, Y. (1989). Proofs and types (Vol. 7). Cambridge Mass: Cambridge university press.
  • Glick, E. (2011). Two methodologies for evaluating intellectualism. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 83(2), 398–434.
  • Glick, E. (2012). Abilities and know-how attributions. In J. Brown and M. Gerken(Eds.), Knowledge Ascriptions, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.120–138.
  • Henke, K. (2010). A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(7), 523–532.
  • Humphreys, G. W. (1983). Reference frames and shape perception. Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 151–196.
  • Humphreys, G. W., & Quinlan, P. T. (1988). Priming effects between two-dimensional shapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(2), 203–220.
  • Jeannerod, M. (1997). The cognitive neuroscience of action. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers, Inc.
  • Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor cognition: What actions tell the self. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9(6), 718–727.
  • Keane, M. M., Gabrieli, J. D., Fennema, A. C., Growdon, J. H., & Corkin, S. (1991). Evidence for a dissociation between perceptual and conceptual priming in Alzheimer‘s disease. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105(2), 326.
  • Knowlton, B. J., & Foerde, K. (2008). Neural representations of nondeclarative memories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 62–67.
  • Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., & Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science, 273(5280), 1399.
  • Kulvicki, J. (2007). What is what it’s like? Introducing perceptual modes of presentation. Synthese, 156(2), 205–229.
  • Lande, K. (2018a). Parts of Perception (PhD Dissertation). University of California, Los Angeles.
  • Lande, K. (2018b). The Perspectival Character of Perception. The Journal of Philosophy, 115(4), 187–214.
  • Lascarides, A. and N. Asher (2004) Imperatives in Dialogue, In P. Kuehnlein, H. Rieser and H. Zeevat (Eds.), The Semantics and Pragmatics of Dialogue for the New Millenium, Benjamins
  • Laurence, S., & Margolis, E. (1999). Concepts and cognitive science. In S. Laurence and E. Margolis (Eds.), Concepts: Core Readings, Boston: MIT Press. 3–81.
  • Levy, N. (2017). Embodied savoir faire: Knowledge-how requires motor representations. Synthese, 194(2), 511–530.
  • Lewis, D. (1994). Reduction of mind. In S. Guttenplan (Ed.), Companion to the philosophy of mind (pp. 412–431). Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Lewis, R. L., Vera, A. H., & Howes, A. (2004). A constraint-based approach to understanding the composition of skill. In M. C. Lovett, C. D. Schunn, C. Lebiere, P. Munro(Eds.), ICCM Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Cognitive Modeling (pp. 148–153) Mahvah NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
  • Lex, H., Schütz, C., Knoblauch, A., & Schack, T. (2015). Cognitive representation of a complex motor action executed by different motor systems. Minds and Machines, 25(1), 1–15.
  • Lupyan, G., & Clark, A. (2015). Words and the world: Predictive coding and the language-perception-cognition interface. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(4), 279–284.
  • Machery, E. (2009). Doing without concepts. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Margolis, E., & Laurence, S. (2014). Concepts. In E. N. Zalta Ed., The stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Retrieved from URL=<https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2014/entries/concepts/>
  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation. Into. New york, NY: WH Freeman.
  • McCloskey, M. (2009). Visual reflections: A perceptual deficit and its implications. New york, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Millikan, R. G. (1984). Language, thought, and other biological categories: new foundations for realism. Boston Mass: MIT press.
  • Milner, B. (1962). Physiologie de l’hippocampe. In Passouant, P. (Ed.), Cen. Natl. Rech. Sci; Les troubles de la memoire acompagnant des lesions hippocampiques bilaterales (pp. 257–272). Paris: Éditions Recherche Scientifique.
  • Milner, B. (1965). Memory disturbance after bilateral hippocampal lesions. In P. M. Milner & S. Glickman (Eds.), Cognitive processes and the brain (pp. 97–111). Princeton, N.J.: Van Nostrand.
  • Moschovakis, Y. N. (1994). Sense and denotation as algorithm and value. Lecture Notes in Logic, 2, 210–249.
  • Mulligan, N. W. (1997). Attention and implicit memory tests: The effects of varying attentional load on conceptual priming. Memory & Cognition, 25(1), 11–17.
  • Muskens, R. (2005). Sense and the computation of reference. Linguistics and Philosophy, 28(4), 473–504.
  • Mylopoulos, M., & Pacherie, E. (2016). Intentions and motor representations: The interface challenge. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 1–20.
  • Mylopoulos, M, & Pacherie, E. (2017). Intentions and motor representations: The interface challenge. Review Of Philosophy and Psychology, 8(2), 317–336.
  • Nagel, J. (2013). 10. Knowledge as a mental state. Oxford Studies in Epistemology, 4, 273.
  • Nanay, B. (2013). Between perception and action. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Neander, K. (2017). A mark of the mental: In defense of informational teleosemantics. Cambridge, Massachusetts; London, England: MIT Press.
  • Noe, A. (2005). Against intellectualism. Analysis, 65(4), 278–290.
  • Pacherie, E. (2011). Nonconceptual representations for action and the limits of intentional control. Social Psychology, 42(1), 67–73.
  • Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision science: Photons to phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
  • Papineau, D. (1984). Representation and explanation. Philosophy Of Science, 51(4), 550–572. doi:10.1086/289205
  • Pavese, C. (2013). The unity and scope of knowledge (Dissertation). Rutgers University.
  • Pavese, C. (2015a). Knowing a rule. Philosophical Issues, 25(1), 165–188.
  • Pavese, C. (2015b). Practical senses. Philosophers’ Imprint, 15(29), 1–25.
  • Pavese, C. (2017a). Know-how and gradability. Philosophical Review, 126(3), 345–383.
  • Pavese, C. (2017b). A theory of practical meaning. Philosophical Topics, 45(2), Fall, 85–116.
  • Pavese, C. (2018). Know-how, action, and luck. Synthese.
  • Pavese, C. (forthcoming). Probabilistic knowledge in action. Analysis.
  • Peacocke, C. (1992). A study of concepts. Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press.
  • Peacocke, C. (1998). Nonconceptual content defended. Philosophy and phenomenological research, 58(2), 381–388.
  • Peacocke, C. (2001). Does Perception Have a Nonconceptual Content? The Journal Of Philosophy, 98(5), 239–264.
  • Platts, M. (1979). Ways of meaning. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
  • Porter, J., Craven, B., Khan, R. M., Chang, S.-J., Kang, I., Judkewitz, B., … Sobel, N. (2007). Mechanisms of scent-tracking in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 10(1), 27–29.
  • Prinz, J. J. (2004). Furnishing the mind: Concepts and their perceptual basis. Cambridge,MA: MIT press.
  • Ramsey, F. (1931). General propositions and causality. In  D. H. Mellor (Ed.), The foundations: Essays in Philosophy, Logic, Mathematics and Economics. (pp. 133–51) London, UK: Routledge
  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2000). Cortical mechanisms subserving object grasping and action recognition: A new view on the cortical motor functions. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 539–552). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2008). Mirrors in the brain: How our minds share actions, emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Rock, I. (1973). Orientation and form. Cambridge,MA: Academic Press.
  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.
  • Rossetti, Y. (2001). Implicit perception in action: Short-lived motor representation of space. In P. G. Grossenbacher (Ed.), Finding consciousness in the brain: A neurocognitive approach. Advances in consciousness research (pp. 133–181). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.
  • Roy, S., & Park, N. W. (2010). Dissociating the memory systems mediating complex tool knowledge and skills. Neuropsychologia, 48(10), 3026–3036.
  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London, UK: Hutchinson
  • Sakai, K, Kitaguchi, K, & Hikosaka, O. (2003). Chunking during human visuomotor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 152(2), 229–242. doi:10.1007/s00221-003-1548-8
  • Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82(4), 225.
  • Schmidt, R. A. (2003). Motor schema theory after 27 years: Reflections and implications for a new theory. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74(4), 366–375.
  • Schulte, P. (2019). Naturalizing the content of desire Philosophical Studies. 176(1), 161–174.
  • Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.
  • Siegel, S. (2011). The contents of visual experience. New york, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Singley, M. K., & Anderson, J. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill (Vol. 9). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Squire, L., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1988). Memory: Brain systems and behavior. Trends in Neurosciences, 11, 170–175.
  • Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99(2), 195.
  • Squire, L. R. (2009). Memory and Brain Systems: 1969–2009. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(41), 12711–12716.
  • Squire, L. R., & Kandel, E. R. (2003). Memory: From mind to molecules. New York, N.Y.: Owl Books.
  • Squire, L. R., & Wixted, J. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 259–288.
  • Squire, L. R., & Wixted, J. (2016). Remembering. In Groes, Sebastian (Eds.), Memory in the twenty-first century: New critical perspectives from the arts, humanities, and sciences (pp. 251–262). Hampshire, England: Palgrave MacMillan.
  • Stalnaker, R. (1998). What might nonconceptual content be? Philosophical Issues, 9, 339–352.
  • Stanley, J. (2011). Know how Oxford U.P. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Stanley, J., & Krakauer, J. W. (2013). Motor skill depends on knowledge of facts. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 503.
  • Stanley, J., & Williamson, T. (2001). Knowing How. Journal of Philosophy, 98(8), 411–444.
  • Stevens, J. A. (2005). Interference effects demonstrate distinct roles for visual and motor imagery during the mental representation of human action. Cognition, 95(3), 329–350.
  • Sutton, J. (2007). Batting, habit, and memory: The embodied mind and the nature of skill. Sport in Society, 10, 763–786.
  • Taatgen, N. A. (2013). The nature and transfer of cognitive skills. Psychological Review, 120(3), 439.
  • Trappenberg, T. (2009). Fundamentals of computational neuroscience. Oxford UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist, 40(4), 385.
  • Verwey, W. B. (1996). Buffer loading and chunking in sequential keypressing. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 22, 544–562.
  • Verwey, W. B. (2001). Concatenating familiar movement sequences: The versatile cognitive processor. Acta Psychologica, 106, 69–95.
  • Verwey, W. B. (2010). Diminished motor skill development in elderly: Indications for limited motor chunk use. Acta Psychologica, 134, 206–214.
  • Verwey, W. B., Abrahamse, E. L., Ruitenberg, M. F., Jiménez, L., & de Kleine, E. (2011). Motorskill learning in the middle-aged: Limited development of motor chunks and explicit sequence knowledge. Psychological Research, 75(5), 406–422.
  • Williamson, T. (2000). Knowledge and its limits. Oxford UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Wolpert, D. M. (1997). Computational approaches to motor control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(6), 209–216.
  • Wolpert, D. M., Diedrichsen, J., & Flanagan, J. R. (2011). Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(12), 739–751.
  • Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Current Biology, 11(18), R729–R732.
  • Wolpert, D. M., & Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1212–1217.
  • Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269(5232), 1880–1882.
  • Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11, 1317–1329.
  • Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., & Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(9), 338–347.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.