787
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

An eye movement based reading intervention in lexical and segmental readers with acquired dyslexia

, , , , &
Pages 833-867 | Received 10 Jul 2013, Accepted 31 Mar 2014, Published online: 09 May 2014

REFERENCES

  • Ablinger, I., & Domahs, F. (2009). Improved single-letter identification after whole-word traning in pure alexia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 19, 340–363. doi: 10.1080/09602010802204000
  • Ablinger, I., Huber, W., Schattka, K., & Radach, R. (2013). Recovery in a letter-by-letter reader: More efficiency at the expense of normal reading strategy. Neurocase, 19, 236–255. iFirst; (doi10.1080/13554794.2012.667119). doi: 10.1080/13554794.2012.667119
  • Arguin, M., & Bub, D. N. (1994). Pure alexia: Attempted rehabilitation and its implications for interpretation of the deficit. Brain and Language, 47, 233–268. doi: 10.1006/brln.1994.1051
  • Beaton, A., & Davis, N. (2007). Semantic errors in deep dyslexia: Does orthographic depth matter? Cognitive Neuropsychologia, 24, 312–323. doi: 10.1080/02643290701251957
  • Beeson, P. M. (1998). Treatment for letter-by-letter reading: A case study. In A. L. Holland (Ed.), Approaches to the treatment of aphasia (pp. 153–177). Singular Publishing Group.
  • Behrmann, M., Black, S. E., & Bub, D. (1990). The evolution of pure alexia: A longitudinal study of recovery. Brain and Language, 39, 405–427. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(90)90148-A
  • Behrmann, M., & McLeod, J. (1995). Rehabilitation for pure alexia: efficacy of therapy and implications for models of normal word recognition. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 5, 149–180. doi: 10.1080/09602019508520179
  • Behrmann, M., Shomstein, S. S., Black, S. E., & Barton, J. J. (2001). The eye movements of pure alexic patients during reading and nonreading tasks. Neuropsychologia, 39, 983–1002. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00021-5
  • Berndt, R. S., Haendiges, A. N., Mitchum, C. C., & Wayland, S. C. (1996). An investigation of nonlexical reading. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 763–801. doi: 10.1080/026432996381809
  • Bohn, C., & Stadie, N. (2005). Unterstützen perzeptuelle Schlüsselmerkmale die ganzheitliche Wortverarbeitung? Die Sprachheilarbeit, 50, 212–220.
  • Bowes, K., & Martin, N. (2007). Longitudinal study of reading and writing rehabilitation using a bigraph–biphone correspondence approach. Aphasiology, 21, 687–701. doi: 10.1080/02687030701192117
  • Buchanan L., McEwen, S., Westbury, C., & Libben, G. (2003). Semantics and semantic errors: Implicit access to semantic information from words and nonwords in deep dyslexia. Brain and Language, 84, 65–83. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00521-7
  • Cherney, L. R. (2004). Aphasia, alexia and oral reading. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 11, 2–36.
  • Coltheart, M. (1980). Deep dyslexia: A review of the syndrome. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 22–48). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
  • Coltheart, M. (1996). Phonological dyslexia: Past and future issues. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 749–762. doi: 10.1080/026432996381791
  • Coltheart, M. (2006). Acquired dyslexias and the computational modelling of reading. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 96–109. doi: 10.1080/02643290500202649
  • Coltheart, M. (2012). Dual-route theories of reading aloud. In J. Adelman (Ed.), Visual word recognition. Volume 1 (pp. 3–27). New York: Psychology Press.
  • Coltheart, M., & Byng, S. (1989). A treatment of surface dyslexia. In X. Seron & G. Deloche (Eds.), Cognitive approaches in neuropsychological rehabilitation (pp. 159–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204
  • Coslett, H. (2000). Acquired dyslexia. Seminars in Neurology, 20, 419–426. doi: 10.1055/s-2000-13174
  • Crisp, J., Howard, D., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012). More evidence for a continuum between phonological and deep dyslexia: Novel data from three measures of direct orthography-to-phonology translation. Aphasiology, 25, 615–641. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2010.541470
  • Crisp, J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). The continuum between phonological and deep dyslexia: The keys are in phonology and semantics. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 348–362. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.3.348
  • De Partz, M. (1986). Re-education of a deep dyslectic patient: Rationale of the methods and results. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 149–177. doi: 10.1080/02643298608252674
  • Friedman, R. B. (1996). Recovery from deep alexia to phonological alexia: Points on a continuum. Brain and Language, 52, 114–128. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0006
  • Friedman, R. B., & Alexander, M. P. (1984). Pictures, images and pure alexia: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 9–23. doi: 10.1080/02643298408252014
  • Friedman, R. B., & Nitzberg Lott, S. (2000). Rapid word identification in pure alexia is lexical but not semantic. Brain and Language, 72, 219–237. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2286
  • Friedman, R. B., & Nitzberg Lott, S. (2002). Successful blending in a phonological reading treatment for deep alexia. Aphasiology, 16, 355–372. doi: 10.1080/02687040143000627
  • Friedman, R. B., & Robinson, S. R. (1991). Whole-word training therapy in a stable surface alexic patient: It works. Aphasiology, 5, 521–527. doi: 10.1080/02687039108248557
  • Gonzalez Rothi, L. J., & Moss, S. (1992). Alexia without agraphia: Potential for model assisted therapy. Clinical Communication Disorders, 2, 11–18.
  • Greenwald, M. L. (2000). The acquired dyslexias. In S. E. Nadeau, B. A. Crosson, & G. L. Gonzalez-Rothi (Eds.), Aphasia and language: Theory to Practice (pp. 159–183). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Hebbema, J. D. F., Hermans, J., & van den Broek, K. (1974). A stepwise discriminant analysis program using density estimation. In G. Bruckmann (Ed.), Proceedings in computational statstics (pp. 101–110). Wien, Germany: Physica-Verlag.
  • Hillis, A., & Caramazza, A. (1991). Mechanisms for accessing lexical representations for output: Evidence from a category-specific semantic deficit. Brain and Language, 40, 106–144. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(91)90119-L
  • Hillis, A., & Caramazza, A. (1995). Converging evidence for the interaction of semantic and phonological information in accessing lexical representations for spoken output. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 187–227. doi: 10.1080/02643299508251996
  • Houghton, G., & Zorzi, M. (2003). Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual-route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 115–162. doi: 10.1080/02643290242000871
  • Howard, D. (1986). Beyond randomized controlled trials: The case for effective case studies of effects of treatment in aphasia. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 21, 89–102. doi: 10.3109/13682828609018546
  • Huber, W. (2002). Alexie und agraphie. In W. Hartje, & K. Poeck (Eds.), Klinische Neuropsychologie (pp. 169–188). Stuttgart, Germany: Thieme.
  • Huber, W., Poeck, K., Weniger, D., & Willmes, K. (1983). Der Aachener Aphasie Test (AAT). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
  • Huber, W., Poeck, K., & Willmes, K. (1985). The Aachen Aphasia Test. In F. C. Rose (Ed.), Progress in Aphasiology (pp. 291–303). New York: Raven.
  • Huber, W., Springer, L., & Willmes, K. (1993). Approaches to aphasia therapy in Aachen. In A. Holland & M. Forbes (Eds.), World perspectives on aphasia (pp. 55–86). San Diego, CA: Singular.
  • Inhoff, A. W., & Radach, R. (1998). Definition and computation of oculomotor measures in the study of cognitive processes. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp. 29–53). Oxford, Elsevier.
  • Johnson, R. L., & Rayner, K. (2007). Top-down and bottom-up effects in pure alexia: Evidence from eye movements. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2246–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.026
  • Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D., & Pynte, J. (Eds.) (2000). Reading as a perceptual process. Oxford: Elsevier.
  • Kiran, S., Thompson, C. K., & Hashimoto, N. (2001). Training grapheme to phoneme conversion in patients with oral reading and naming deficits: A model-based approach. Aphasiology, 15, 855–876. doi: 10.1080/02687040143000258
  • Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Graham, N. L. (2000). Acquired phonological and deep dyslexia. Neurocase, 6, 141–178. doi: 10.1080/13554790008402767
  • Leff, A., & Behrmann, M. (2008). Treatment of reading impairment after stroke. Current Opinion in Neurology, 21, 644–648. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e3283168dc7
  • Lott, S. N., Friedman, R. B., & Linebaugh, C. W. (1994). Rationale and efficacy of a tactile-kinaesthetic treatment for alexia. Aphasiology, 8, 181–195. doi: 10.1080/02687039408248650
  • Marshall, J., & Newcombe, F. C. (1973). Patterns of paralexia, a psycholinguistic approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 175–199. doi: 10.1007/BF01067101
  • Matthews, C. (1991). Serial processing and the “phonetic route”: Lessons learned in the functional reorganization of deep dyslexia. Journal of Communication Disorders, 24, 21–39. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(91)90031-D
  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407.
  • Mitchum, C., & Berndt, R. (1991). Diagnosis and treatment of the non-lexical route in acquired dyslexia: An illustration of the cognitive neuropsychological approach. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 6, 103–137. doi: 10.1016/0911-6044(91)90003-2
  • Morton, J., & Patterson, K. E. (1980). A new attempt at an interpretation, or, an attempt at a new interpretation. In M. Coltheart, K. E. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.
  • Newcombe, F., & Marshall, J. C. (1981). On psycholinguistic classifications of the acquired dyslexias. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 31, 29–46. doi: 10.1007/BF02658599
  • Nickels, L. (1992). The autocue? Self-generated phonemic cues in the treatment of a disorder of reading and naming. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 935–980. doi: 10.1080/02643299208252057
  • Nitzberg, L. S. & Friedman, R. B. (1999). Can treatment for pure alexia improve letter-by-letter reading speed without sacrificing accuracy? Brain and Language, 67, 188–201. doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2054
  • Nuthmann, A., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Mislocated fixations during reading and the inverted optimal viewing position effect. Vision Research, 45, 2201–2217. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.02.014
  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–110. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
  • Patterson, K. E., Marshall, J. C., & Coltheart, M. (1985). Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Plaut, D. C. (1999). A connectionist approach to word reading and acquired dyslexia: Extension to sequential processing. In M. H. Christiansen and N. Chater (Eds.), Connectionist psycholinguistics. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Rewiew, 103, 56–115.
  • Radach, R., & Kempe, V. (1993). An individual analysis of initial fixation positions in reading. In G. d'Ydewalle & J. Van Rensbergen (Eds.), Perception and cognition: Advances in eye movement research (pp. 213–226). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
  • Radach, R., Inhoff, A. W., & Heller, D. (2004). Orthographic regularity gradually modulates saccade amplitudes in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 27–51. doi: 10.1080/09541440340000222
  • Radach, R., & Kennedy, A. (2004). Theoretical perspectives on eye movements in reading: Past controversies, current issues, and an agenda for future research. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 3–26. doi: 10.1080/09541440340000295
  • Radach, R., & Kennedy, A. (2013). Eye movements in reading: Some theoretical context. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 429–452. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.750676
  • Radach, R., & McConkie, G. W. (1998). Determinants of fixation positions in reading. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Eye guidance in reading and scence perception (pp. 77–100). Oxford: Elsevier.
  • Rapcsak, S., Henry, M., Teague, S., Carnahan, S., & Beeson, P. (2007). Do dual-route models accurately predict reading and spelling performance in individuals with acquired alexia and agraphia? Neuropsychologia, 45, 2519–2524. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.019
  • Rapp, B., Eppstein, C., & Tainturier, M. (2002). The integration of information across lexical and sublexical processes in spelling. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 1–29. doi: 10.1080/0264329014300060
  • Rastle, K., Tyler, L., K., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2006). New evidence for morphological errors in deep dyslexia. Brain and Language, 97, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.10.003
  • Rayner, K. (1979). Eye guidance in reading: Fixation locations within words. Perception, 8, 21–30. doi: 10.1068/p080021
  • Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1457–1506. doi: 10.1080/17470210902816461
  • Rayner, K., & Johnson, R. L. (2005). Letter-by-letter acquired dyslexia is due to the serial encoding of letters. Psychological Science, 16, 530–534. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01570.x
  • Reilly, R., & Radach, R. (2006). Some empirical tests of an interactive activation model of eye movement control in reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 7, 34–55. doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2005.07.006
  • Sage, K., Hesketh, A., & Ralph, M. A. (2005). Using errorless learning to treat letter-by-letter reading: Contrasting word versus letter-based therapy. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 15, 619–642. doi: 10.1080/09602010443000155
  • Schattka, K. I., Radach, R., & Huber, W. (2010). Eye movement correlates of acquired central dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2959–2973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.005
  • Scott, C., & Byng, S. (1989). Computer assisted remediation of a homophone comprehension disorder in surface dyslexia. Aphasiology, 3, 301–320. doi: 10.1080/02687038908248996
  • Shallice, T., Warrington, E. K., & McCarthey, R. (1983). Reading without semantics. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 111–138. doi: 10.1080/14640748308402120
  • Ska, B., Garneau-Beaumont, D., Chesneau, S., & Damien, B. (2003). Diagnosis and rehabilitation attempt of a patient with acquired deep dyslexia. Brain and Cognition, 53, 339–363. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00143-X
  • Stadie, N., & Rilling, E. (2006). Evaluation of lexically and nonlexically based treatment in a deep dyslexic. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 643–672. doi: 10.1080/02643290500538364
  • Tang, S., Reilly, R., & Vorstius, C. (2012). EyeMap: A software system for visualizing and analyzing eye movement data in reading. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 420–438. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0156-y
  • Weekes, B., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Surface dyslexia and surface dysgraphia: Treatment studies and their theoretical implications. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 277–315. doi: 10.1080/026432996382033
  • Willmes, K. (1985). An approach to analyzing a single subject's scores obtained in a standardized test with application to the Aachen aphasia test (AAT). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 331–352. doi: 10.1080/01688638508401268
  • Yampolsky, S., & Waters, G. (2002). Treatment of single word oral reading in an individual with deep dyslexia. Aphasiology, 16, 455–471. doi: 10.1080/02687030244000068

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.