327
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

How do students improve their value-based learning with task experience?

&
Pages 928-942 | Received 03 Nov 2013, Accepted 18 Jun 2014, Published online: 23 Jul 2014

REFERENCES

  • Ariel, R., & Dunlosky, J. (2013). When do learners shift from habitual to agenda-based processes when selecting items for study? Memory & Cognition, 41, 416–428. doi:10.3758/s13421-012-0267-4
  • Ariel, R., Dunlosky, J., & Bailey, H. (2009). Agenda-based regulation of study-time allocation: When agendas override material-based monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 432–447. doi:10.1037/a0015928
  • Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., Howe, M. L., & Kevershan, J. (1990). The last shall be first: How memory strength affects children's retrieval. Psychological Science, 1, 247–252. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00208.x
  • Castel, A. D. (2008). The adaptive and strategic use of memory by older adults: Evaluative processing and value-directed remembering. In A. S. Benjamin & B. H. Ross (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 48, pp. 225–270). London: Academic Press.
  • Castel, A. D., Balota, D. A., & McCabe, D. P. (2009). Memory efficiency and the strategic control of attention at encoding: Impairments of value-directed remembering in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 23, 297–306. doi:10.1037/a0014888
  • Castel, A. D., Benjamin, A. S., Craik, F. I. M., & Watkins, M. J. (2002). The effects of aging on selectivity and control in short-term recall. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1078–1085. doi:10.3758/BF03194325
  • Castel, A. D., Farb, N. A. S., & Craik, F. I. M. (2007). Memory for general and specific value information in younger and older adults: Measuring the limits of strategic control. Memory & Cognition, 35, 689–700. doi:10.3758/BF03193307
  • Castel, A. D., Humphreys, K. L., Lee, S. S., Galván, A., Balota, D. A., & McCabe, D. P. (2011). The development of memory efficiency and value-directed remembering across the life span: A cross-sectional study of memory and selectivity. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1553–1564. doi:10.1037/a0025623
  • Castel, A. D., Lee, S. S., Humphreys, K. L., & Moore, A. N. (2011). Memory capacity, selective control, and value-directed remembering in children with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuropsychology, 25(1), 15–24. doi:10.1037/a0020298
  • Castel, A. D., Murayama, K., Friedman, M. C., McGillivray, S., & Link, I. (2013). Selecting valuable information to remember: Age-related differences and similarities in self-regulated learning. Psychology and Aging, 28, 232–242. doi:10.1037/a0030678
  • Credé, M., & Kuncel, N. R. (2008). Study habits, skills, and attitudes: The third pillar supporting collegiate academic performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 425–453. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00089.x
  • Dunlosky, J., & Ariel, R. (2011). Self-regulated learning and the allocation of study time. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 54, pp. 103–140). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Dunlosky, J., & Thiede, K. W. (1998). What makes people study more? An evaluation of factors that affect self-paced study. Acta Psychologica, 98(1), 37–56. doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(97)00051-6
  • Dunlosky, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2004). Causes and constraints of the shift-to-easier-materials effect in the control of study. Memory & Cognition, 32, 779–788. doi:10.3758/BF03195868
  • Koriat, A., Ackerman, R., Adiv, S., Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2014). The effects of goal-driven and data-driven regulation on metacognitive monitoring during learning: A developmental perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 386–403. doi:10.1037/a0031768
  • Le Ny, J.-F., Denhière, G., & Le Taillanter, D. (1972). Regulation of study-time and interstimulus similarity in self-paced learning conditions. Acta Psychologica, 36, 280–289. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(72)90012-1
  • McGillivray, S., & Castel, A. D. (2011). Betting on memory leads to metacognitive improvement by younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 137–142. doi:10.1037/a0022681
  • Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2005). A region of proximal learning model of study time allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 463–477. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.12.001
  • Nelson, T. O., & Leonesio, R. J. (1988). Allocation of self-paced study time and the “labor-in-vain effect.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 676–686. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.14.4.676
  • Soderstrom, N. C., & McCabe, D. P. (2011). The interplay between value and relatedness as bases for metacognitive monitoring and control: Evidence for agenda-based monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1236–1242. doi:10.1037/a0023548
  • Thiede, K. W., & Dunlosky, J. (1999). Toward a general model of self-regulated study: An analysis of selection of items for study and self-paced study time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1024–1037. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.1024
  • Watkins, M. J., & Bloom, L. C. (1999). Selectivity in memory: An exploration of willful control over the remembering process. Unpublished manuscript.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.