1,818
Views
9
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Can implicit measures detect source information in crime-related amnesia?

ORCID Icon, , , , & ORCID Icon
Pages 1019-1029 | Received 24 Jul 2017, Accepted 12 Feb 2018, Published online: 18 Feb 2018

References

  • Baker, G. A., Hanley, J. R., Jackson, H. F., Kimmance, S., & Slade, P. (1993). Detecting the faking of amnesia: Performance differences between simulators and patients with memory impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 668–684. doi: 10.1080/01688639308402588
  • Bayen, U. J., Murnane, K., & Erdfelder, E. (1996). Source discrimination, item detection, and multinomial models of source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 197–215. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.22.1.197
  • Bylin, S. (2002). How does repeated simulation of memory impairment affect genuine memory performance? Psychology, Crime and Law, 8, 265–288. doi: 10.1080/10683160208401819
  • Bylin, S., & Christianson, SÅ. (2002). Characteristics of malingered amnesia: Consequences of withholding vs. distorting information on later memory of a crime event. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 7, 45–61. doi: 10.1348/135532502168379
  • Byrne, R. M. J. (2005). The rational imagination: How people create alternatives to reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Byrne, R. M. J. (2016). Counterfactual thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 135–157. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033249
  • Christianson, S. A., & Bylin, S. (1999). Does simulating amnesia mediate genuine forgetting for a crime event? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 495–511. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(199912)13:6<495::aid-acp615>3.0.co;2-0
  • Christianson, S. A., & Merckelbach, H. (2004). Crime-related amnesia as a form of deception. In P. A. Granhag, & L. A. Strömwall (Eds.), The detection of deception in forensic contexts (pp. 195–225). Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511490071
  • Chrobak, Q. M., & Zaragoza, M. S. (2008). Inventing stories: Forcing witnesses to fabricate entire fictitious events leads to freely reported false memories. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 1190–1195. doi: 10.3758/pbr.15.6.1190
  • Cima, M., Merckelbach, H. L. G. J., Nijman, H., Knauer, E., & Hollnack, S. (2002). I can’t remember your honor: Offenders who claim amnesia. German Journal of Psychiatry, 5, 24–34.
  • Curci, A., Lanciano, T., Maddalena, C., Mastandrea, S., & Sartori, G. (2015). Flashbulb memories of the Pope’s resignation: Explicit and implicit measures across differing religious groups. Memory, 23, 529–544. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.908923
  • De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them. In The handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11–28). doi: 10.4135/9781412976237.n2
  • De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 347–368. doi: 10.1037/a0014211
  • Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: A three-component model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 379–386. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.001
  • Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2008). The effects of unitization on familiarity-based source memory: Testing a behavioral prediction derived from neuroimaging data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 730–740. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.730
  • Evans, C. (2006). What violent offenders remember of their crime: Empirical explorations. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 508–518. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2006.01833.x
  • Evans, C., Ehlers, A., Mezey, G., & Clark, D. M. (2007). Intrusive memories in perpetrators of violent crime: Emotions and cognitions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 134–144. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.1.134
  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225
  • Giger, P., Merten, T., Merckelbach, H., & Oswald, M. (2010). Detection of feigned crime-related amnesia: A multi-method approach. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 10, 440–463. doi: 10.1080/15228932.2010.489875
  • Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27. doi:10.1037//0033-295x.102.1.4 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4
  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1464 doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464
  • Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2001). Health of the implicit association test at age 3. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 85–93. doi.org/10.1026//0949-3946.48.2.85
  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197
  • Helm, R. K., Ceci, S. J., & Burd, K. A. (2016). Can implicit associations distinguish true and false eyewitness memory? Development and preliminary testing of the IATe. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 34, 803–819. doi: 10.1002/bsl.2272
  • Hicks, J. L., Marsh, R. L., & Ritschel, L. (2002). The role of recollection and partial information in source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 503–508. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.503
  • Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385. doi: 10.1177/0146167205275613
  • Iverson, G. L. (1995). Qualitative aspects of malingered memory deficits. Brain Injury, 9, 35–40. doi: 10.3109/02699059509004569
  • Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541. doi: 10.1016/0749-596x(91)90025-f
  • Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G., & Raye, C. L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 371–376. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.117.4.371
  • Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3
  • Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Foley, H. J., & Foley, M. A. (1981). Cognitive operations and decision bias in reality monitoring. American Journal of Psychology, 94, 37–64. doi: 10.2307/1422342
  • Lanciano, T., Curci, A., Mastandrea, S., & Sartori, G. (2013). Do automatic mental associations detect a flashbulb memory? Memory, 21, 482–493. doi:10.1080/09658211.2014.894221 doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.740050
  • Lindsay, D. S. (2008). Source monitoring. In H. L. Roediger, III (Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory. Vol. 2 of learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (pp. 325–348). Oxford: Elsevier.
  • McWilliams, K., Goodman, G. S., Lyons, K. E., Newton, J., & Avila-Mora, E. (2014). Memory for child sexual abuse information: Simulated memory error and individual differences. Memory & Cognition, 42, 151–163. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0345-2
  • Merckelbach, H., & Christianson, S. A. (2007). Amnesia for homicide as a form of malingering. In S. A. Christianson (Ed.), Offenders’ memories of violent crimes (pp. 165–190). Chichester, England: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9780470713082.ch7
  • Mitchell, K. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2009). Source monitoring 15 years later: What have we learned from fMRI about the neural mechanisms of source memory? Psychological Bulletin, 135, 638–677. doi: 10.1037/a0015849
  • Mollison, M. V., & Curran, T. (2012). Familiarity in source memory. Neuropsychologia, 50, 2546–2565. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.027
  • Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). The implicit association test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Automatic processes in social thinking and behavior (pp. 265–292). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Pyszora, N. M., Barker, A. F., & Kopelman, M. D. (2003). Amnesia for criminal offences: A study of life sentence prisoners. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 14, 475–490. doi: 10.1080/14789940310001599785
  • Roese, N. J., & Epstude, K. (2017). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking: New evidence, new challenges, new insights. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 1–79. doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2017.02.001
  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1997). Counterfactual thinking: The intersection of affect and function. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 1–59. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60015-5
  • Sartori, G., Agosta, S., Zogmaister, C., Ferrara, S. D., & Castiello, U. (2008). How to accurately detect autobiographical events. Psychological Science, 19, 772–780. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02156.x
  • Schacter, D. L. (1986). Amnesia and crime: How much do we really know? American Psychologist, 41, 286–295. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.41.3.286
  • Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.13.3.501
  • Schacter, D. L. (1992). Understanding implicit memory: A cognitive neuroscience approach. American Psychologist, 47, 559–569. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.47.4.559 doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.47.4.559
  • Smith, D., & Resnick, P. (2007). Amnesia and competence to stand trial. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 35, 541–543.
  • Takarangi, M. K., Strange, D., & Houghton, E. (2015). Event familiarity influences memory detection using the aIAT. Memory (Hove, England), 23, 453–461. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.902467
  • Takarangi, M. K., Strange, D., Shortland, A. E., & James, H. E. (2013). Source confusion influences the effectiveness of the autobiographical IAT. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 1232–1238. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0430-3
  • Terraciano, A., McCrae, R. R., & CostaJr, P. T. (2003). Factorial and construct validity of the Italian positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 19, 131–141. doi: 10.1027//1015-5759.19.2.131
  • Thompson, C. P., Skowronski, J. J., Larsen, S. F., & Betz, A. L. (1996). Autobiographical memory: Remembering what and remembering when. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. doi.org/10.4324/9780203773741
  • Tobias, B. A., Kihlstrom, J. F., & Schacter, D. L. (1992). Emotion and implicit memory. In The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (pp. 67–92). doi: 10.4324/9781315807454
  • Van Oorsouw, K., & Merckelbach, H. (2004). Feigning amnesia undermines memory for a mock crime. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 505–518. doi: 10.1002/acp.999
  • Van Oorsouw, K., & Merckelbach, H. (2006). Simulating amnesia and memories of a mock crime. Psychology, Crime & Law, 12, 261–271. doi: 10.1080/10683160500224477
  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
  • Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126. doi:10.1037//0033-295x.107.1.101 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.101
  • Windows, M. R., & Smith, G. P. (2009). SIMS – Structured inventory of malingered symptomatology. Adattamento italiano a cura di La Marca S., Rigoni D., Sartori G., & Lo Priore C. Giunti OS, Firenze: 2011.
  • Yonelinas, D. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441–517. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2002.2864
  • Yonelinas, A. P., Kroll, N. E. A., Dobbins, I. G., & Soltani, M. (1999). Recognition memory for faces: When familiarity supports associative recognition judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 654–661. doi: 10.3758/bf03212975
  • Zangrossi, A., Agosta, S., Cervesato, G., Tessarotto, F., & Sartori, G. (2015). “I didn’t want to do it!” The detection of past intentions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 608–608. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00608