162
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Phonological false recognition produced by bottom-up automatic activation in young and older people

, ORCID Icon, , & ORCID Icon
Pages 528-535 | Received 22 May 2018, Accepted 02 Oct 2018, Published online: 11 Oct 2018

References

  • Alameda, J. R., & Cuetos, F. (1995). Diccionario de frecuencias de las unidades linguísticas del castellano. [Frequency Dictionary of Castellan Linguistic units]. Oviedo: Universidad de Oviedo.
  • Algarabel, S., Escudero, J., Mazón, J. F., Pitarque, A., Fuentes, M., Peset, V., & Lacruz, L. (2009). Familiarity-based recognition in the young, healthy elderly, mild cognitive impaired and Alzheimer’s patients⋆. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2056–2064. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.016
  • Algarabel, S., Pitarque, A., Tomás, J. M., & Mazón, J. F. (2010). Explorations of familiarity produced by words with specific combinations of letters. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 265–285. doi: 10.1080/09541440902767818
  • Ballou, M. R., & Sommers, M. S. (2008). Similar phenomena, different mechanisms: Semantic and phonological false memories are produced by independent mechanisms. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1450–1459. doi: 10.3758/MC.36.8.1450
  • Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., Wright, R., & Mojardin, A. H. (2003). Recollection rejection: False-memory editing in children and adults. Psychological Review, 110, 762–784. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.762
  • Budson, A. E., Sullivan, A. L., Daffner, K. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2003). Semantic versus phonological false recognition in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 51, 251–261. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00030-7
  • Calvillo, D. P., & Parong, J. A. (2016). The misinformation effect is unrelated to the DRM effect with and without a DRM warning. Memory (Hove, England), 24, 324–333. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1005633
  • Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22. doi: 10.1037/h0046671
  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
  • Devitt, A. L., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). False memories with age: Neural and cognitive underpinnings. Neuropsychologia, 91, 346–359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.030
  • Dewhurst, S. A., & Robinson, C. A. (2004). False memories in children: Evidence for a shift from phonological to semantic associations. Psychological Science, 15, 782–786. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00756.x
  • Finley, J. R., Sungkhasettee, V. W., Roediger III, H. L., & Balota, D. A. (2017). Relative contributions of semantic and phonological associates to over-additive false recall in hybrid DRM lists. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 154–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.07.006
  • Folstein, M, Folstein, S, & McHugh, P. (1975). Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
  • Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38, 833–848. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.7.833
  • Gallo, D. A., Sullivan, A. L., Daffner, K. R., Schacter, D. L., & Budson, A. E. (2004). Associative recognition in Alzheimer’s disease: Evidence for impaired recall-to-reject. Neuropsychology, 18, 556–563. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.556
  • Holliday, R. E., & Weekes, B. S. (2006). Dissociated developmental trajectories for semantic and phonological false memories. Memory (Hove, England), 14, 624–636. doi: 10.1080/09658210600736525
  • Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3
  • Koen, J. D., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2014). The effects of healthy aging, amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease on recollection and familiarity: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review, 24, 332–354. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9266-5
  • Koen, J. D., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2016). Recollection, not familiarity, decreases in healthy ageing: Converging evidence from four estimation methods. Memory (Hove, England), 24, 75–88. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.985590
  • Loftus, E. F. (2003). Make-believe memories. American Psychologist, 5, 864–873.
  • Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (1980). The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition, 8, 1–71. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(80)90015-3
  • Miller, M. B., Guerin, S. A., & Wolford, G. L. (2011). The strategic nature of false recognition in the DRM paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1228–1235.
  • Old, S. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 23, 104–118. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.104
  • Parkin, A., Ward, J., Squires, E., Furbear, H., Clark, A., & Townshend, J. (2001). Data-driven recognition memory: A new technique and some data on age differences. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 812–819. doi: 10.3758/BF03196222
  • Pitarque, A., Meléndez, J., Sales, A., Mayordomo, T., Satorres, E., Escudero, J., & Algarabel, S. (2016). The effects of healthy aging, amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease on recollection, familiarity and false recognition, estimated by an associative process-dissociation recognition procedure. Neuropsychologia, 91, 29–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.07.010
  • Rankin, J. L., & Kausler, D. H. (1979). Adult age differences in false recognitions. Journal of Gerontology, 34, 58–65. doi: 10.1093/geronj/34.1.58
  • Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 1–75. doi: 10.1016/1041-6080(95)90031-4
  • Roediger III, H. L. & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814.
  • Roediger, H. L., Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 385–407. doi: 10.3758/BF03196177
  • Schoemaker, D., Gauthier, S., & Pruessner, J. C. (2014). Recollection and familiarity in aging individuals with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: A literature review. Neuropsychology Review, 24, 313–331. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9265-6
  • Sommers, M. S., & Huff, L. M. (2003). The effects of age and dementia of the Alzheimer’s type on phonological false memories. Psychology and Aging, 18, 791–806. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.791
  • Sommers, M. S., & Lewis, B. P. (1999). Who really lives next door: Creating false memories with phonological neighbors. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 83–108. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2614
  • Swannell, E. R., & Dewhurst, S. A. (2012). Phonological false memories in children and adults: Evidence for a developmental reversal. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 376–383. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.11.003
  • Tse, C. S., Li, Y., & Trammell, N. W. (2011). Dissociative effects of phonological vs. semantic associates on recognition memory in the Deese/Roediger–McDermott paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 137, 269–279. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.013
  • Wallace, P. W., Stewart, M. T., & Malone, C. P. (1995). Recognition memory errors produced by implicit activation of word candidates during the processing of spoken words. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 417–439. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1995.1019
  • Watson, J. M., Balota, D. A., & Roediger, H. L. (2003). Creating false memories with hybrid lists of semantic and phonological associates: Over-additive false memories produced by converging associative networks. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 95–118. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00019-6
  • Watson, J. M., Balota, D. A., & Sergent-Marshall, S. D. (2001). Semantic, phonological, and hybrid veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in individuals with Dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychology, 15, 254–267. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.15.2.254
  • Zhu, B., Chen, C., Loftus, E. F., Lin, C., & Dong, Q. (2013). The relationship between DRM and misinformation false memories. Memory & Cognition, 41, 832–838. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0300-2

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.