191
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Seeing Where the Stone Is Thrown by Observing a Point-Light Thrower: Perceiving the Effect of Action Is Enabled by Information, Not Motor Experience

&

REFERENCES

  • Abernethy, B., & Zawi, K. (2007). Pickup of essential kinematics underpins expert perception of movement patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39(5), 353–367.
  • Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11(9), 1109–1116.
  • Al-abood, S. A., Davids, K., & Bennett, S. J. (2001). Specificity of task constraints and effects of visual demonstrations and verbal instructions in directing learners' search during skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(3), 295–305.
  • Beardsworth, T., & Buckner, T. (1981). The ability to recognize oneself from a video recording of one's movements without seeing one's body. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 19–22.
  • Bingham, G. P. (1987). Kinematic form and scaling: Further investigations on the visual perception of lifted weight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(2), 155–177.
  • Bingham, G. P. (1995). Dynamics and the problem of visual event recognition. In T. R. Port & T. Van Gelder (Eds.), Mind as motion: Dynamics, behavior and cognition (pp. 403–448). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Bingham, G. P., Rosenblum, L. D., & Schmidt, R. C. (1995). Dynamics and the orientation of kinematic forms in visual event recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(6), 1473–1493.
  • Bingham, G. P., & Wickelgren, E. A. (2008). Events and actions as dynamically molded spatiotemporal objects: A critique of the motor theory of biological motion perception. In T. F. Shipley & J. M. Zacks (Eds.), Understanding events: From perception to action (pp. 266–279). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Blake, R. (1993). Cats perceive biological motion. Psychological Science, 4(1), 54–57.
  • Bouquet, C. A., Gaurier, V., Shipley, T., Toussaint, L., & Blandin, Y. (2007). Influence of the perception of biological or non-biological motion on movement execution. Journal of Sports Sciences, 25(5), 519–530.
  • Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16(19), 1905–1910.
  • Cañal-Bruland, R., & Schmidt, M. (2009). Response bias in judging deceptive movements. Acta Psychologica, 130(3), 235–240.
  • Cañal-Bruland, R., van der Kamp, J., & van Kesteren, J. (2010). An examination of motor and perceptual contributions to the recognition of deception from others' actions. Human Movement Science, 29(1), 94–102.
  • Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16(1), 69–74.
  • Cutting, J. E., Proffitt, D. R., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1978). A biomechanical invariant for gait perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4(3), 357–372.
  • Decety, J., & Grèzes, J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(5), 172–178.
  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593–609.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1972). A theory of direct visual perception. In J. Royce & W. Rozenboom (Eds.), The psychology of knowing (pp. 77–89). New York, NY: Gordon & Breach.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Original work published 1979)
  • Giese, M. A., & Poggio, T. (2003). Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 179–192.
  • Grossman, E. D., & Blake, R. (2002). Brain areas active during visual perception of biological motion. Neuron, 35(6), 1167–1175.
  • Horn, R. R., Williams, A. M., & Scott, M. A. (2002). Learning from demonstrations: The role of visual search during observational learning from video and point-light models. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20(3), 253–269.
  • Horn, R. R., Williams, A. M., Scott, M. A., & Hodges, N. J. (2005). Visual search and coordination changes in response to video and point-light demonstrations without KR. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37(4), 265–274.
  • Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286(5449), 2526–2528.
  • Isenhower, R. W., Richardson, M. J., Carello, C., Baron, R. M., & Marsh, K. (2010). Affording cooperation: Embodied constraints, dynamics and action-scaled invariance in joint lifting. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 342–347.
  • Jacobs, D. M., Runeson, S., & Michaels, C. F. (2001). Learning to visually perceive the relative mass of colliding balls in globally and locally constrained task ecologies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(5), 1019–1038.
  • Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 14(2), 201–211.
  • Jokisch, D., Daum, I., & Troje, N. F. (2006). Self recognition versus recognition of others by biological motion: Viewpoint-dependent effects. Perception, 35, 911–920.
  • Kilner, J., Hamilton, A. F., & Blakemore, S. J. (2007). Interference effect of observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion. Social Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 158–166.
  • Kilner, J., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S. J. (2003). An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13(6), 522–525.
  • Knoblich, G., & Flach, R. (2001). Predicting the effects of actions: Interactions of perception and action. Psychological Science, 12(6), 467–472.
  • Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Recognition of self-generated actions from kinematic displays of drawing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 456–465.
  • Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 210–220.
  • Mark, L. S. (2007). Perceiving the actions of other people. Ecological Psychology, 19(2), 107–136.
  • Mather, G., & Murdoch, L. (1994). Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Series B. Biological Sciences, 258(1353), 273–279.
  • Muchisky, M. M., & Bingham, G. P. (2002). Trajectory forms as a source of information about events. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(1), 15–31.
  • Munzert, J., Hohmann, T., & Hossner, E. J. (2010). Discriminating throwing distances from point-light displays with masked ball flight. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(2), 247–264.
  • Pavlova, M. A. (2012). Biological motion processing as a hallmark of social cognition. Cerebral Cortex, 22(5), 981–995.
  • Pavlova, M., Krägeloh-Mann, I., Sokolov, A., & Birbaumer, N. (2000). Recognition of point-light biological motion displays by young children. Perception, 30(8), 925–933.
  • Pavlova, M., Staudt, M., Sokolov, A., Birbaumer, N., & Krägeloh-Mann, I. (2003). Perception and production of biological movement in patients with early periventricular brain lesions. Brain, 126(3), 692–701.
  • Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). The visual perception of human and animal motion in point-light displays. Social Neuroscience, 4(4), 332–346.
  • Pollick, F. E., Kay, J. W., Heim, K., & Stringer, R. (2005). Gender recognition from point-light walkers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(6), 1247–1265.
  • Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.
  • Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational bias for person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 585–615.
  • Scheerer, E. (1984). Motor theories of cognitive structure: Historical review. In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and motor processes (pp. 77–98). Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag.
  • Serino, A., De Filippo, L., Casavecchia, C., Coccia, M., Shiffrar, M., & Làdavas, E. (2010). Lesions to the motor system affect action perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(3), 413–426.
  • Shipley, T. F. (2003). The effect of object and event orientation on perception of biological motion. Psychological Science, 14(4), 377–380.
  • Smith, J. E. K. (1982). Simple algorithms for M-alternative-forced-choice calculations. Perception & Psychophysics, 31(1), 95–96.
  • Snippe, H. P., & Koenderink, J. J. (1994). Discrimination of geometric angle in the fronto-parallel plane. Spatial Vision, 8(3), 309–328.
  • Sumi, S. (1984). Upside-down presentation of the Johansson moving light-spot pattern. Perception, 13(3), 283–286.
  • Todd, J. T. (1981). Visual information about moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(4), 795–810.
  • Tremoulet, P. D., & Feldman, J. (2000). Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception, 29(8), 943–952.
  • Troje, N. F., & Aust, U. (2013). What do you mean with “direction”? Local and global cues to biological motion perception in pigeons. Vision Research, 79, 47–55.
  • Troje, N. F., Sadr, J., Geyer, H., & Nakayama, K. (2006). Adaptation aftereffects in the perception of gender from biological motion. Journal of Vision, 6(8), 850–857.
  • Troje, N. F., & Westhoff, C. (2006). The inversion effect in biological motion perception: Evidence for a “life detector”? Current Biology, 16(8), 821–824.
  • Troje, N. F., Westhoff, C., & Lavrov, M. (2005). Person identification from biological motion: Effects of structural and kinematic cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(4), 667–675.
  • Tye-Murray, N., Spehar, B. P., Myerson, J., Hale, S., & Sommers, M. S. (2013). Reading your own lips: Common-coding theory and visual speech perception. Psychonomic Bulletin & Reviews, 20(1), 115–119.
  • Vallortigara, G., Regolin, L., & Marconato, F. (2005). Visually inexperienced chicks exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patterns. PLoS Biology, 3(7), e208. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208
  • Warren, W. H., Kim, E. E., & Husney, R. (1987). The way the ball bounces: Visual and auditory perception of elasticity and control of the bounce pass. Perception, 16, 309–336.
  • Wickelgren, E. A., & Bingham, G. P. (2004). Perspective distortion of trajectory forms and perceptual constancy in visual event identification. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 629–641.
  • Wickelgren, E. A., & Bingham, G. P. (2008). Trajectory forms as information for visual event recognition: 3D perspectives on path shape and speed profile. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(2), 266–278.
  • Williams, A. M., Hodges, N. J., North, J. S., & Barton, G. (2006). Perceiving patterns of play in dynamic sport tasks: Investigating the essential information underlying skilled performance. Perception, 35(3), 317–332.
  • Wilson, A. D., Bingham, G. P., & Craig, J. C. (2003). Proprioceptive perception of phase variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(6), 1179.
  • Yoon, J., & Johnson, S. C. (2009). Biological motion displays elicit social behavior in 12-month-olds. Child Development, 80(4), 1069–1075.
  • Zhu, Q., Lu, J. D., & Wilson, A. D. (2012). Expert release control in response to changing spatial properties of a remote target. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, S34, 149.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.